Infant Nap Frequency Reflects Cognitive Needs

Summary: A new study found that the frequency and duration of naps in infants are an indication of their cognitive needs. According to the research, infants with smaller vocabularies and poorer cognitive abilities tend to nap more frequently.

The study, conducted during the 2020 lockdown, used data from 463 infants aged between eight months and three years. The researchers suggest that caregivers should respect an infant’s sleep patterns, as they are a reflection of their individual cognitive requirements.

Key Facts:

  1. The research team found that infants with smaller vocabularies and lower scores in measures of executive function, nap more frequently.
  2. The structure of daytime sleep in infants can serve as an indicator of cognitive development.
  3. The researchers suggest that caregivers should accommodate a child’s mental age, rather than chronological age, to determine a child’s sleep needs.

Source: Economic and Social Research Council

Infants who nap a lot have smaller vocabularies and poorer cognitive skills—according to new research from the University of East Anglia.

Parents the world over are prone to worry about their children getting either too little or too much sleep.

This shows a napping child.
What we found is that the structure of daytime sleep is an indicator of cognitive development. Credit: Neuroscience News

But a new study accepted for publication in JCPP Advances reveals that some children are more efficient at consolidating information during sleep, so they nap less frequently.

Meanwhile others, usually those with fewer words and poorer cognitive skills, need to nap more frequently.

The research team say that reducing naps for these children will not improve brain development, and that they should be allowed to nap as frequently and for as long as they need.

Lead researcher Dr. Teodora Gliga said, “There is a lot of parental anxiety around sleep. Parents worry that their kids don’t nap as much as expected for their age—or nap too frequently and for too long.

“But our research shows that how frequently a child naps reflects their individual cognitive need. Some are more efficient at consolidating information during sleep, so they nap less frequently.

“Children with smaller vocabularies or a lower score in a measure of executive function, nap more frequently.

“Young children will naturally nap for as long as they need and they should be allowed to do just that,” she added.

The research team studied 463 infants aged between eight months and three years during lockdown in 2020.

Parents were surveyed about their children’s sleep patterns, their ability to focus on a task, keep information in their memory, and the number of words that they understood and could say.

They also asked parents about their socio-economic status—including their postcode, income, and education—and about the amount of screen time and outdoors activities their child engaged in.

Dr. Gliga said, “Lockdown gave us an opportunity to study children’s intrinsic sleep needs because when children are in childcare, they rarely nap as much as they need to.

“Because nurseries were closed, it meant less disturbance to the children’s natural sleep patterns. None of the children taking part were attending day care.

“What we found is that the structure of daytime sleep is an indicator of cognitive development.

“Infants with more frequent but shorter naps than expected for their age had smaller vocabularies, and worse cognitive function.

“We also found that this negative association between vocabulary and frequency of naps was stronger in older children,” she added.

“While the majority of parents told us that their child’s sleep was unaffected by lockdown, parents from lower socio-economic backgrounds were more likely to report a worsening in sleep.

“Screen time increased during lockdown and outdoor activities decreased but these did not explain differences in children’s sleep.

“Previous work suggested that caregivers should encourage frequent naps, in pre-school children.

“Our findings suggest that children have different sleep needs—some children may drop naps earlier because they don’t need them anymore. Others may still need to nap past three years of age.

“In the U.K., preschools enrolling 3 to 5-year-olds have no provisions for napping. Caregivers should use a child’s mental age and not chronological age to ascertain a child’s sleep needs,” she added.

About this sleep and neurodevelopment research news

Author: Press Office
Source: Economic and Social Research Council
Contact: Press Office – Economic and Social Research Council
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access.
More frequent naps are associated with lower cognitive development in a cohort of eight- to 38- month-old children, during the COVID-19 pandemic” by Teodora Gliga et al. JCPP Advances


Abstract

More frequent naps are associated with lower cognitive development in a cohort of eight- to 38- month-old children, during the COVID-19 pandemic

Background. How often a child naps, during infancy, is believed to reflect both intrinsic factors, i.e. the need of an immature brain to consolidate information soon after it is acquired, and environmental factors. To date, the difficulty to account for important environmental factors that interfere with a child’s sleep needs (e.g., attending childcare) has clouded our ability to understand the role of intrinsic drivers of napping frequency.

Methods. Here we investigate sleep patterns in association with two measures of cognitive ability, vocabulary size, measured with the Oxford-Communicative Development Inventory (N = 298) and cognitive executive functions, measured with the Early Executive Functions Questionnaire (N = 463), in a cohort of 8- to 38-month-olds. Importantly, because of the social distancing measures imposed during the Covid-19 Spring 2020 lockdown, in the UK, these children did not access daycare settings.

Results. We find that children with more frequent but shorter naps than expected for their age had lower concurrent receptive vocabularies, lower cognitive executive functions and a slower increase in expressive vocabulary from spring to winter 2020, when age, sex and SES were accounted for. The negative association between vocabulary and frequency of naps was stronger in older children.

Conclusions. These findings suggest that the structure of daytime sleep is an indicator of cognitive development and highlight the importance of considering environmental perturbations and age when investigating developmental correlates of sleep.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.