Like Adults, Children Show Bias in Attributing Mental States to Others

Summary: A new study reveals an early origin for certain social phenomena, including dehumanizing and bias between social groups.

Source: APS.

Young children are more likely to attribute mental states to characters that belong to the same group as them relative to characters that belong to an outside group, according to findings published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science. The study shows that 5- and 6-year-olds were more likely to describe interactions between two characters in terms of what they were thinking and feeling when the characters had the same gender or geographic origin as them.

“We found that young children were selective in the way they attribute mental states – they were less likely to spontaneously refer to the mind of individuals belonging to a different group,” says researcher Niamh McLoughlin of the University of York. “Our research suggests that, at least from the age of 5, children demonstrate a bias that might be similar to dehumanization – ascribing fewer mental abilities and uniquely human traits to others. In addition, this work illustrates that there are situations in which children are more or less motivated to reason about the minds of other people.”

Previous research has shown that adults tend to dehumanize people who belong to social groups or categories – for example, race, gender, socioeconomic status – that are different from their own. These “outsiders” are seen as having less intelligence, rationality, and emotional depth relative to those who belong to the in-group. McLoughlin and University of York co-author Harriet Over were interested in investigating the developmental origins of this phenomenon.

“Our aim was to examine whether young children also exhibited this bias with relation to mental state attribution,” says McLoughlin.

Adapting an established technique, the researchers showed a total of 128 5- and 6-year-olds animations of a big triangle and a small triangle that seemed to interact, with one triangle seemingly coaxing or surprising the other.

Each child saw two videos. In one, the triangles were described as having the same gender or coming from the same town as the participant; in the other, the triangles were described as having a different gender or coming from a far-away country. The researchers chose to examine gender because it’s a category that children are particularly sensitive to, while they decided to look at geographic origin because of its relevance to current social and political debates.

The researchers asked the participants to describe what happened and to rate how much they liked the group discussed in each video.

Using a predetermined coding scheme, the researchers counted any words that described a character’s thoughts, desires, emotions, intentions, or current states as mental-state terms.

The data showed that 6 year-olds used more mental-state words overall and a more diverse range of these words compared with children who were one year younger, a finding that highlights the ongoing development of theory-of-mind processes in early childhood.

Image shows a group of kids.
The data showed that 6 year-olds used more mental-state words overall and a more diverse range of these words compared with children who were one year younger, a finding that highlights the ongoing development of theory-of-mind processes in early childhood. NeuroscienceNews.com image is adapted from the APS news release.

More importantly, both 5- and 6-year-olds used more mental-state terms when they believed the characters had the same gender or home town as opposed to when they had a different one. Six-year-olds also used more diverse mental-state words in describing characters from the same group relative to those from a different group.

This group-based bias extended to direct ratings: Participants also preferred individuals who belonged to their own gender and geographic group.

These findings hint at early origins for social phenomena including bias between social groups and dehumanization, the researchers argue.

“We plan to use this research as the base for future work examining the social consequences of biased mental-state attribution, such as the extent to which children help members of an outgroup,” says McLoughlin. “We hope that this work can ultimately inform research-led interventions that aim to foster positive intergroup relations.”

About this neuroscience research article

Funding: This research was funded by Economic and Social Research Council Grant ES/K006702/1 (to H. Over).

Source: Anna Mikulak – APS
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is adapted from the APS news release.
Original Research: Abstract for “Young Children Are More Likely to Spontaneously Attribute Mental States to Members of Their Own Group” by Niamh McLoughlin and Harriet Over in Psychological Science. Published online August 22 2017 doi:10.1177/0956797617710724

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]APS “Like Adults, Children Show Bias in Attributing Mental States to Others.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 22 August 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/children-mental-state-bias-7353/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]APS (2017, August 22). Like Adults, Children Show Bias in Attributing Mental States to Others. NeuroscienceNew. Retrieved August 22, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/children-mental-state-bias-7353/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]APS “Like Adults, Children Show Bias in Attributing Mental States to Others.” https://neurosciencenews.com/children-mental-state-bias-7353/ (accessed August 22, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Young Children Are More Likely to Spontaneously Attribute Mental States to Members of Their Own Group

We investigated whether young children were more likely to spontaneously attribute mental states to members of their own social group than to members of an out-group. We asked 5- and 6-year-old children to describe the actions of interacting geometric shapes and manipulated whether the children believed these shapes represented their own group or another group. Children of both ages spontaneously used mental-state words more often in their description of in-group members compared with out-group members. Furthermore, 6-year-olds produced a greater diversity of mental-state terms when talking about their own social group. These effects held across two different social categories (based on gender and geographic location). This research has important implications for understanding a broad range of social phenomena, including dehumanization, intergroup bias, and theory of mind.

“Young Children Are More Likely to Spontaneously Attribute Mental States to Members of Their Own Group” by Niamh McLoughlin and Harriet Over in Psychological Science. Published online August 22 2017 doi:10.1177/0956797617710724

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.