Aging Memories May Not Be ‘Worse,’ Just ‘Different’

Summary: Aging may alter neural processes for remembering continuous real-world experiences and segmenting memories.

Source: WUSTL

“Memory is the first thing to go.”

Everyone has heard it, and decades of research studies seem to confirm it: While it may not always be the first sign of aging, some faculties, including memory, do get worse as people age.

It may not be that straightforward.

Zachariah Reagh, assistant professor of psychological and brain sciences in Arts & Sciences at Washington University in St. Louis, looked at the brain activity of older people not by requiring them to recite a group of words or remember a string of numbers. Instead, Reagh looked at a “naturalistic approach,” one that more closely resembled real-world activities.

He found that brain activity in older adults isn’t necessarily quieter when it comes to memory.

“It’s just different,” he said.

The study results were published today in the journal Nature Communications.

Common tests of memory involve a person’s ability to remember a string of words, count backward, or recognize repeated images. “How many times do you suspect a 75-year-old is going to have to remember, ‘tree, apple, cherry, truck?'” asked Reagh, first author on the paper with Angelique Delarazan, Alexander Garber and Charan Ranganath, all of University of California, Davis.

Instead, he used a data set from the Cambridge Center for Aging and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) that included functional MRI (fMRI) scans of people watching an 8-minute movie. “There were no specific instructions, or a ‘gotcha’ moment,” Reagh said. “They just got to kick back, relax and enjoy the film.”

But while they may have been relaxing, the subjects’ brains were hard at work recognizing, interpreting and categorizing events in the movies. One particular way people categorize events is by marking boundaries—where one event ends and another begins.

An “event” can be pretty much anything, Reagh said. “This conversation, or a component of it, for example. We take these meaningful pieces and extract them out of a continuous stream.”

And what constitutes a boundary is actually consistent among people.

“If you and I watch the same movie, and we are given the instruction to press a button when we feel one meaningful unit has ended, you and I will be much more similar in our responses than we are different,” Reagh said.

When looking at the fMRI results—which use changes in blood flow and blood oxygen to highlight brian activity—older adults showed similarly increased activity as a control group at the boundaries of events. That’s not to say that brains of all ages are processing the information similarly.

“It’s just different,” Reagh said. “In some areas, activity goes down and, in some, it actually goes up.”

Overall activity did decline pretty reliably across ages 18-88, Reagh said, and when grouped into “younger, middle aged, and older,” there was a statistically reliable drop in activity from one group to another.

“But we did find a few regions where activity was ramped up across age ranges,” he said. “That was unexpected.”

Much of the activity he was interested in is in an area of the brain referred to as the posterior medial network—which includes regions in the midline and toward the backside of the brain. In addition to memory, these areas are heavily involved in representing context and situational awareness. Some of those areas showed decreased activity in the older adults.

“We do think the differences are memory-related,” Reagh said. At the boundaries, they saw differences in the levels of activity in the hippocampus that was related to memory in a different measurement—”story memory,” he called it.

“There might be a broad sense in which the hippocampus’s response to event boundaries predicts how well you are able to parse and remember stories and complex narratives,” no matter one’s age, Reagh said.

But for older adults, closer to the front of the brain, particularly the medial prefrontal cortex, things were looking up.

This shows an older woman
He found that brain activity in older adults isn’t necessarily quieter when it comes to memory. Image is in the public domain.

Activity in that area of the brain was ramped up in older adults. This area is implicated in broad, schematic knowledge—what it’s like to go to a grocery store as opposed to a particular grocery store.

“What might be happening is as older adults lose some responsiveness in posterior parts of the brain, they may be shifting away from the more detailed contextual information,” Reagh said. But as activity levels heighten in the anterior portions, “things might become more schematic. More ‘gist-like.'”

In practice, this might mean that a 20-year-old noting an event boundary in a movie might be more focused on the specifics—what specific room are the characters in? What is the exact content of the conversation? An older viewer might be paying more attention to the broader picture—What kind of room are the characters in? Have the characters transitioned from a formal dinner setting to a more relaxed, after-dinner location? Did a loud, tense conversation resolve into a friendly one?

“Older adults might be representing events in different ways, and transitions might be picked up differently than, say, a 20-year-old,” Reagh said.

“An interesting conclusion one could draw is maybe healthy older adults aren’t ‘missing the picture.” It’s not that the info isn’t getting in, it’s just it’s getting in differently.”

About this memory research article

Source:
WUSTL
Contacts:
Brandie Jefferson – WUSTL
Image Source:
The image is in the public domain.

Original Research: Open access
“Aging alters neural activity at event boundaries in the hippocampus and Posterior Medial network” by Zachariah M. Reagh et al. Nature Communications.


Abstract

Aging alters neural activity at event boundaries in the hippocampus and Posterior Medial network

Recent research has highlighted a role for the hippocampus and a Posterior Medial cortical network in signaling event boundaries. However, little is known about whether or how these neural processes change over the course of healthy aging. Here, 546 cognitively normal participants 18–88 years old viewed a short movie while brain activity was measured using fMRI. The hippocampus and regions of the Posterior Medial network show increased activity at event boundaries, but these boundary-evoked responses decrease with age. Boundary-evoked activity in the posterior hippocampus predicts performance on a separate test of memory for stories, suggesting that hippocampal activity during event segmentation may be a broad indicator of individual differences in episodic memory ability. In contrast, boundary-evoked responses in the medial prefrontal cortex and middle temporal gyrus increase across the age range. These findings suggest that aging may alter neural processes for segmenting and remembering continuous real-world experiences.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.