Summary: Smell influences how children evaluate strange faces, a new study reports.
Source: Monell Chemical Senses Center.
Monell study reveals children use odor information to help evaluate strange faces.
New research from the Monell Center reveals that children begin using olfactory information to help guide their responses to emotionally-expressive faces at about five years of age. The findings advance understanding of how children integrate different types of sensory information to direct their social behavior.
“Even though we may not be aware of it, the sense of smell influences how adults process emotional and social information to guide their decisions and behavior. Our findings establish that, beginning at the age of five, smell also influences children’s emotional decisions,” said cognitive neuroscientist Valentina Parma, PhD, one of the study’s authors.
In the study, published online ahead of print in Developmental Science, 140 children between three and eleven years old were invited to participate in the research while visiting a local children’s museum. Each child was exposed to one of three odors, either rose, fish, or blank, for three seconds. Immediately afterwards, the child saw a screen containing photographs of two faces, one happy and the other disgusted, and was asked to select one. Both facial expressions were from the same person. Afterward, the children rated the pleasantness of the odor.
The findings showed that children under the age of five tended to choose the happy face, regardless of the associated odor or how they rated its pleasantness.
However, beginning at age five, the odor influenced the children’s decision of which face to select. Specifically, the older children based their selection on whether the visual and olfactory cues were emotionally similar; for example, the happy face was selected more frequently when paired with an odor rated as pleasant. Similarly, exposure to the unpleasant fish odor increased the likelihood of choosing the disgusted face.
“Now that we know that children as young as five years old use smells to make emotionally-based decisions, it may be possible to use this information in educational settings to guide social behavior,” said Parma.
Moving forward, the researchers intend to explore whether this same developmental path applies to children with autism spectrum disorder. If so, the sense of smell might represent a useful tool to complement social and emotional treatment options.
Parma also commented on the value of conducting the research on site at Philadelphia’s Please Touch Museum, a children’s museum focused on creating learning opportunities through play.
“Taking the research outside the lab benefitted the museum, the local community and the researchers,” said Parma. “The Please Touch Museum was able to provide children and parents with the opportunity to interact with scientists and learn about the research process. In turn, the research team established that we could conduct the research outside the laboratory setting without sacrificing methodological standards. This allowed us to enroll and test hundreds of children within a short period of time. It was a win for all involved.”
Also contributing to the research were Monell scientists Johan Lundström, Annachiara Cavazzana, and Christiane Wesarg; and Julia Parish-Morris from the Center for Autism Reseatch at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia. Cavazzana was first author on the paper. Parma, who was a Louise Slade Fellow at Monell at the time the research was conducted, is currently at the Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati (SISSA) in Trieste, Italy.
Funding: The partnership between Monell and the Please Touch Museum was supported through a National Living Laboratory Grant from the National Science Foundation.
Source: Leslie Stein – Monell Chemical Senses Center
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
Original Research: Abstract for “When preschoolers follow their eyes and older children follow their noses: visuo-olfactory social affective matching in childhood” by Annachiara Cavazzana, Christiane Wesarg, Julia Parish-Morris, Johan N. Lundström, and Valentina Parma in Developmental Psychology. Published online November 17 20166 doi:10.1111/desc.12507
[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]Monell Chemical Senses Center. “With Eyes or Nose? How Young Children Use Sensory Cues to Make Social Decisions.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 19 December 20166.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/sensory-cues-social-decisions-5775/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]Monell Chemical Senses Center. (20166, December 19). With Eyes or Nose? How Young Children Use Sensory Cues to Make Social Decisions. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved December 19, 20166 from https://neurosciencenews.com/sensory-cues-social-decisions-5775/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]Monell Chemical Senses Center. “With Eyes or Nose? How Young Children Use Sensory Cues to Make Social Decisions.” https://neurosciencenews.com/sensory-cues-social-decisions-5775/ (accessed December 19, 20166).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]
Abstract
When preschoolers follow their eyes and older children follow their noses: visuo-olfactory social affective matching in childhood
Recognition of emotional facial expressions is a crucial skill for adaptive behavior that most often occurs in a multi-sensory context. Affective matching tasks have been used across development to investigate how people integrate facial information with other senses. Given the relative affective strength of olfaction and its relevance in mediating social information since birth, we assessed olfactory–visual matching abilities in a group of 140 children between the ages of 3 and 11 years old. We presented one of three odor primes (rose, fish and no-odor, rated as pleasant or unpleasant by individual children) before a facial choice task (happy vs. disgusted face). Children were instructed to select one of two faces. As expected, children of all ages tended to choose happy faces. Children younger than 5 years of age were biased towards choosing the happy face, irrespective of the odor smelled. After age 5, an affective matching strategy guided children’s choices. Smelling a pleasant odor predicted the choice of happy faces, whereas smelling the unpleasant or fish odor predicted the choice of disgusted faces. The present study fills a gap in the developmental literature on olfactory-visual affective strategies that affect decision-making, and represents an important step towards understanding the underlying developmental processes that shape the typical social mind.
“When preschoolers follow their eyes and older children follow their noses: visuo-olfactory social affective matching in childhood” by Annachiara Cavazzana, Christiane Wesarg, Julia Parish-Morris, Johan N. Lundström, and Valentina Parma in Developmental Psychology. Published online November 17 20166 doi:10.1111/desc.12507