Why Older People Might Lose Their Way

Summary: Researchers report the firing patterns of grid cells are less stable in older adults. This, they say, could be why older people have more problems with spatial navigation than younger people.

Source: DZNE.

Researchers at the German Center for Neurodegenerative Disease (DZNE) have found a possible explanation for the difficulty in spatial orientation experienced sometimes by elderly people. In the brains of older adults, they detected an unstable activity in an area that is central for spatial navigation. The results are reported in the journal “Current Biology”. In the long term, these findings might open up new ways for detecting Alzheimer’s disease.

To guide us through space in a goal directed manner, the human brain has to process a flood of information, ranging from visual stimuli to cues provided by the muscular system and our sense of balance. Thus, spatial orientation and navigation are among the most complex abilities of the human mind. However, these skills often deteriorate as we grow older, which can severely compromise independence and quality of life.

“When you move around an unfamiliar environment, it is perfectly normal to get lost. Yet, this tends to happen more often to older people. So far, we know very little about the underlying neuronal mechanisms of these navigation problems”, says Matthias Stangl, a researcher at the DZNE’s Magdeburg site and first author of the current publication. “We had the hypothesis that so-called grid cells might be implicated. A major part of the navigational processing is done by these cells. They are specialized neurons located in the brain’s entorhinal cortex. Therefore, we guessed that deficits in grid cell function might be a cause for problems in navigation.”

In virtual reality and in real space

To test this assumption, Stangl and colleagues performed experiments with 41 healthy young and older adults, who were split in two groups: The group of “young adults” consisted of 20 participants aged between 19 and 30 years, whereas the group of “older adults” comprised 21 individuals aged between 63 and 81 years. Both groups included men and women.

One of the experiments combined functional brain imaging (fMRI) and virtual reality: The participants had to navigate through a computer-generated scenery while their brain activity patterns were monitored. A second experiment tested the ability for “path integration”. In this setup, participants moved along predefined curved paths. At intermediate stops, they had to estimate their distance and orientation relative to their starting point, but without being able to see or pinpoint its location. Since this test was carried out in two versions, it took place both in real space and in a virtual environment.

Instabilities in firing patterns

“All things considered, young participants did better in navigation, which is in line with previous studies. However, we found an association between decreased navigational performance and deficits in grid cell activity”, says Prof. Thomas Wolbers, a DZNE senior scientist and supervisor of the current study. “Grid cells fired differently when comparing young and old adults. Specifically, firing patterns were less stable over time in older individuals, which indicates that these brain circuits are compromised in old age. This might be a cause of why many senior people tend to have troubles with spatial navigation.”

spots on floor
Floor pattern as used in one of the virtual reality experiments: This setup tested the ability of young and older adults for spatial orientation. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to DZNE.

Wolbers adds: “Grid cells play a central role not just in navigation but also in other cognitive functions. Therefore, our findings might indicate a key mechanism underlying cognitive deficits in old age. Not only does this provide insights into neurophysiological changes due to aging. It may also help in designing therapies against age-related cognitive decline.”

An early sign for dementia?

While weakening navigational skills might occur in healthy adults, such a decline is also considered as one of the earliest symptoms of Alzheimer’s. “Assessing navigation performance and grid cell function could possibly facilitate early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s and other neurodegenerative disorders”, says Wolbers. “To this end, it would be necessary to develop diagnostic methods that distinguish between an age-related decline in navigational ability and a decline caused by disease. This might be a challenging task. However, our findings lay the foundation for future studies on such topics.”

About this neuroscience research article

Source: Marcus Neitzert – DZNE
Publisher: Organized by NeuroscienceNews.com.
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to DZNE.
Original Research: Open access research in Current Biology.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2018.02.038 

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]DZNE “Why Older People Might Lose Their Way.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 16 March 2018.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/navigation-aging-8652/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]DZNE (2018, March 16). Why Older People Might Lose Their Way. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved March 16, 2018 from https://neurosciencenews.com/navigation-aging-8652/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]DZNE “Why Older People Might Lose Their Way.” https://neurosciencenews.com/navigation-aging-8652/ (accessed March 16, 2018).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Compromised Grid-Cell-like Representations in Old Age as a Key Mechanism to Explain Age-Related Navigational Deficits

Highlights
•Grid-cell-like representations in human entorhinal cortex are compromised in old age
•This effect is predominantly driven by a lack of representational stability over time
•Path integration ability in old age is associated with grid-cell-like representations

Summary
A progressive loss of navigational abilities in old age has been observed in numerous studies, but we have only limited understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying this decline. A central component of the brain’s navigation circuit are grid cells in entorhinal cortex, largely thought to support intrinsic self-motion-related computations, such as path integration (i.e., keeping track of one’s position by integrating self-motion cues). Given that entorhinal cortex is particularly vulnerable to neurodegenerative processes during aging and Alzheimer’s disease, deficits in grid cell function could be a key mechanism to explain age-related navigational decline. To test this hypothesis, we conducted two experiments in healthy young and older adults. First, in an fMRI experiment, we found significantly reduced grid-cell-like representations in entorhinal cortex of older adults. Second, in a behavioral path integration experiment, older adults showed deficits in computations of self-position during path integration based on body-based or visual self-motion cues. Most strikingly, we found that these path integration deficits in older adults could be explained by their individual magnitudes of grid-cell-like representations, as reduced grid-cell-like representations were associated with larger path integration errors. Together, these results show that grid-cell-like representations in entorhinal cortex are compromised in healthy aging. Furthermore, the association between grid-cell-like representations and path integration performance in old age supports the notion that grid cells underlie path integration processes. We therefore conclude that impaired grid cell function may play a key role in age-related decline of specific higher-order cognitive functions, such as spatial navigation.

Feel free to share this DZNE.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.