Early Predictors of Children’s Language Delays

Summary: The specific types of words children learn first can be a predictor of early language difficulties.

Source: University of Miami

In a new study led by Associate Professor Lynn Perry, researchers have found that the specific types of words children know first can be an early predictor of language difficulties.

The paper, “Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long-term language outcomes,” which was recently published in Developmental Science, suggests that the number of words toddlers know that name things that are similar in shape, such as cup, ball, and spoon (i.e., “shape-based nouns”), can differentiate between children who are considered late bloomers and those who have persisting delays months or years later.

“Some children who are late talkers and show initial delays in their vocabulary as toddlers, eventually catch up to have a typically sized vocabulary. We call these children late bloomers,” said Perry, who studies child language development in the Department of Psychology.

“But some late talkers do not seem to catch up, and so far, researchers have been unable to predict early on who is going to be who.

“In our study,” she adds, “we wanted to figure out which children with slow initial vocabulary development would be late bloomers and which might have persisting delays in vocabulary growth.”

Persisting delays could also be an indicator of developmental language disorder (DLD). About seven percent of the population has DLD, but it’s not very well-known compared to other developmental disabilities, according to Perry.

“DLD is characterized by persisting delays and difficulties in language throughout the lifespan, but it’s not typically diagnosed until school-age, because there aren’t any good early predictors for who might catch up and who might need more support,” said collaborating author Sarah Kucker, an assistant professor of psychology at Oklahoma State University.

“This is the goal of our current study—to see if the type of words a child knows in toddlerhood can predict later delays.”

The research team’s prior work suggests that most children with typical vocabulary development learn a lot of shape-based nouns during toddlerhood.

“When children learn a word like ‘cup,’ they figure out that even though there are different cup colors and materials, they all have the same approximate shape, which is the same with spoon, ball, table, and so on,” said Perry.

“Once children learn a lot of these shape-based nouns, they automatically pay attention to shape as they learn new words and can start learning new words more quickly. So we thought knowledge of shape-based nouns might be important to investigate in children who are late talkers.”

This shows a child on a swing
The research team’s prior work suggests that most children with typical vocabulary development learn a lot of shape-based nouns during toddlerhood. Image is in the public domain

The team used extensive datasets of vocabulary development in three groups of over 800 children—those who were late talkers in toddlerhood but had a more typically sized vocabulary months later (late bloomers); those who were late talkers in toddlerhood and continued to have delays in their vocabulary months later (persisting late talkers); and children who had typically sized vocabularies throughout both time periods.

The team used a parent-report measure of children’s vocabulary knowledge when children were about 16 months old and again when they were about 28 months old to assess the proportion of shape-based nouns children knew, controlling for their vocabulary size. At 16 months, those children who would become persisting late talkers knew a smaller proportion of shape-based nouns than late bloomers and typical talkers.

A subset of the participants were followed until they were seven years old to see if any received a diagnosis of DLD. Children who were later were diagnosed with DLD also knew a smaller proportion of shape-based nouns at 16 months than other children.

Perry says the study can be useful for parents, early child educators, and speech language pathologists who work with young children with language delays.

“It’s beneficial to know who might need extra support and what kind of support different children with language delays might need,” she said. “Perhaps, helping late talkers learn more shape-based words in particular, could be helpful for language development as they get older.”

About this language and neurodevelopment research news

Author: Press Office
Source: University of Miami
Contact: Press Office – University of Miami
Image: The image is in the public domain

Original Research: Closed access.
Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes” by Lynn K. Perry et al. Developmental Science


Abstract

Late bloomer or language disorder? Differences in toddler vocabulary composition associated with long‐term language outcomes

Children with delays in expressive language (late talkers) have heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Some are late bloomers who eventually “catch-up,” but others have persisting delays or are later diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD). Early in development it is unclear which children will belong to which group.

We compare the toddler vocabulary composition of late talkers with different long-term outcomes. The literature suggests most children with typical development (TD) have vocabularies dominated by names for categories organized by similarity in shape (e.g., cup), which supports a bias to attend to shape when generalizing names of novel nouns—a bias associated with accelerated vocabulary development.

Previous work has shown that as a group, late talkers tend to say fewer names for categories organized by shape and are less likely to show a “shape bias” than TD children.

Here, in a retrospective analysis of 850 children, we compared the vocabulary composition of groups of toddlers who were late bloomers or persisting late talkers. At Time 1 (13-27 months), the persisting late talkers said a smaller proportion of shape-based nouns than both TD children and late bloomers who “caught up” to typically sized vocabularies months later (18-38-months).

Additionally, children who received a DLD diagnosis between 4 and 7 years said a significantly smaller proportion of shape-based nouns in year two than TD children and children with other diagnoses (e.g., dyslexia).

These findings bring new insight into sources of heterogeneity amongst late talkers and offer a new metric for assessing risk.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.