New Study of the Words “A” And “The” Sheds Light on Language Acquisition

Summary: Researchers suggest that while experience is important for early language acquisition, it does not account for all of a child’s language facility.

Source: MIT.

If you have the chance, listen to a toddler use the words “a” and “the” before a noun. Can you detect a pattern? Is he or she using those two words correctly?

And one more question: When kids start using language, how much of their know-how is intrinsic, and how much is acquired by listening to others speak?

Now a study co-authored by an MIT professor uses a new approach to shed more light on this matter — a central issue in the area of language acquisition.

The results suggest that experience is an important component of early-childhood language usage although it doesn’t necessarily account for all of a child’s language facility. Moreover, the extent to which a child learns grammar by listening appears to change over time, with a large increase occurring around age 2 and a leveling off taking place in subsequent years.

“In this view, adult-like, rule-based [linguistic] development is the end-product of a construction of knowledge,” says Roger Levy, an MIT professor and co-author of a new paper summarizing the study. Or, as the paper states, the findings are consistent with the idea that children “lack rich grammatical knowledge at the outset of language learning but rapidly begin to generalize on the basis of structural regularities in their input.”

The paper, “The Emergence of an Abstract Grammatical Category in Children’s Early Speech,” appears in the latest issue of Psychological Science. The authors are Levy, a professor in MIT’s Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences; Stephan Meylann of the University of California at Berkeley; Michael Frank of Stanford University; and Brandon Roy of Stanford and the MIT Media Lab.

Learning curve

Studying how children use terms such as “a dog” or “the dog” correctly can be a productive approach to language acquisition, since children use the articles “a” and “the” relatively early in their lives and tend to use them correctly. Again, though: Is that understanding of grammar innate or acquired?

Some previous studies have examined this specific question by using an “overlap score,” that is, the proportion of nouns that children use with both “a” and “the,” out of all the nouns they use. When children use both terms correctly, it indicates they understand the grammatical difference between indefinite and definite articles, as opposed to cases where they may (incorrectly) think only one or the other is assigned to a particular noun.

One potential drawback to this approach, however, is that the overlap score might change over time simply because a child might hear more article-noun pairings, without fully recognizing the grammatical distinction between articles.

By contrast, the current study builds a statistical model of language use that incorporates not only child language use but adult language use recorded around children, from a variety of sources. Some of these are publicly available copora of recordings of children and caregivers; others are records of individual children; and one source is the “Speechome” experiment conducted by Deb Roy of the MIT Media Lab, which features recordings of over 70 percent of his child’s waking hours.

The Speechome data, as the paper notes, provides some of the strongest evidence yet that “children’s syntactic productivity changes over development” — that younger children learn grammar from hearing it, and do so at different rates during different phases of early childhood.

Image shows toddlers and the words "a" and "the".
The extent to which a child learns grammar by listening appears to change over time, with a large increase occurring around age 2 and a leveling off taking place in subsequent years, according to a new study. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Jose-Luis Olivares/MIT.

“I think the method starts to get us traction on the problem,” Levy says. “We saw this as an opportunity both to use more comprehensive data and to develop new analytic techniques.”

A work in progress

Still, as the authors note, a second conclusion of the paper is that more basic data about language development is needed. As the paper notes, much of the available information is not comprehensive enough, and thus “likely not sufficient to yield precise developmental conclusions.”

And as Levy readily acknowledges, developing an airtight hypothesis about grammar acquisition is always likely to be a challenge.

“We’re never going to have an absolute complete record of everything a child has ever heard,” Levy says.

That makes it much harder to interpret the cognitive process leading to either correct or incorrect uses of, say, articles such as “a” and “the.” After all, if a child uses the phrase “a bus” correctly, it still might only be because that child has heard the phrase before and likes the way it sounds, not because he or she grasped the underlying grammar.

“Those things are very hard to tease apart, but that’s what we’re trying to do,” Levy says. “This is only really an initial step.”

About this psychology research article

Funding: The researchers acknowledge funding from the U.S. National Institutes of Health.

Source: Peter Dizikes – MIT
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Jose-Luis Olivares/MIT.
Original Research: Abstract for “The Emergence of an Abstract Grammatical Category in Children’s Early Speech” by Stephan C. Meylan, Michael C. Frank, Brandon C. Roy, and Roger Levy in Psychological Science. Published online January 10 2017 doi:10.1177/0956797616677753

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]MIT “New Study of the Words “A” And “The” Sheds Light on Language Acquisition.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 6 April 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/language-acquisition-psychology-6352/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]MIT (2017, April 6). New Study of the Words “A” And “The” Sheds Light on Language Acquisition. NeuroscienceNew. Retrieved April 6, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/language-acquisition-psychology-6352/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]MIT “New Study of the Words “A” And “The” Sheds Light on Language Acquisition.” https://neurosciencenews.com/language-acquisition-psychology-6352/ (accessed April 6, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

The Emergence of an Abstract Grammatical Category in Children’s Early Speech

How do children begin to use language to say things they have never heard before? The origins of linguistic productivity have been a subject of heated debate: Whereas generativist accounts posit that children’s early language reflects the presence of syntactic abstractions, constructivist approaches instead emphasize gradual generalization derived from frequently heard forms. In the present research, we developed a Bayesian statistical model that measures the degree of abstraction implicit in children’s early use of the determiners “a” and “the.” Our work revealed that many previously used corpora are too small to allow researchers to judge between these theoretical positions. However, several data sets, including the Speechome corpus—a new ultra-dense data set for one child—showed evidence of low initial levels of productivity and higher levels later in development. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that children lack rich grammatical knowledge at the outset of language learning but rapidly begin to generalize on the basis of structural regularities in their input.

“The Emergence of an Abstract Grammatical Category in Children’s Early Speech” by Stephan C. Meylan, Michael C. Frank, Brandon C. Roy, and Roger Levy in Psychological Science. Published online January 10 2017 doi:10.1177/0956797616677753

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.