depressed woman
The data used for the study was collected via sleep tests with both normal sleepers and sleepers with insomnia. NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.

Why People With Insomnia Don’t Know They’re Asleep

Summary: People with insomnia have greater brain activity in regions associated with conscious awareness during NREM sleep, researchers report.

Source: Brigham Young University.

When you can’t get to sleep at night, you might explain it to someone as your brain not being able to shut off.

While your brain never truly shuts off, when you do fall asleep, your brain sends inhibitory neurons that help reduce conscious awareness to get to a point of deep sleep. Normal sleepers often feel like they’ve fallen asleep before their brain is in a scientifically-defined state of sleep, but people with insomnia aren’t so lucky.

A recent study by BYU psychology professor Daniel Kay published in Sleep suggests a dysfunction in the inhibition process could be what causes those with insomnia to have a hard time fully falling asleep.

“Previous studies found that patients with insomnia appear to be asleep, their eyes are closed and their brain is in a characteristic sleep pattern, but you wake them up and guess what they are more likely to tell you? ‘I was awake,’” Kay said.

This problem has traditionally been characterized by sleep scientists as sleep misperception. Kay, however, argues that that term is based on the assumption that sleep is categorical, either being asleep or being awake, and that when you’re asleep you don’t have consciousness.

“I don’t think that’s necessarily true,” Kay said. “I think you can be consciously aware and your brain be in a sleep pattern. The question is: What role does conscious awareness have in our definition of sleep?”

The data used for the study was collected via sleep tests with both normal sleepers and sleepers with insomnia.

To help the participants feel comfortable enough to fall asleep, they slept at the lab for two nights before the study. They were in a quiet room with a comfortable bed. The participants were monitored with polysomnography, the gold-standard objective measure of sleep, and once their brain-wave patterns had been in a state of sleep for at least 10 minutes, a radioactive tracer was injected in their arm. The tracer, attached to glucose molecules, was taken into active brain neurons. After 20 minutes, the researchers woke up the participants and took a scan of their brain. The images represented a snapshot of regional brain activity that occurred while the participants were asleep.

Kay and his colleagues found that when patients reported being awake longer than polysomnography measured, they had greater activity in regions of the brain associated with conscious awareness during non-rapid eye movement sleep.

When good sleepers reported going to sleep before polysomnographic sleep occurred, they too had greater brain activity in the same regions.

Kay suggested that while both patients with insomnia and normal sleepers may experience an inhibition process while falling asleep, patients with insomnia may not perceive being asleep until their brain has a large increase in inhibitory activity in brain regions involved in conscious awareness. Good sleepers, likewise, may experience going to sleep before the objective measure due to greater inhibitory processes in consciousness centers of the brain.

Kay hopes to take these findings and research potential treatments for insomnia.

“In patients with insomnia, processes involved in reducing conscious awareness during sleep may be impaired,” Kay said. “One of the strategies for targeting these processes may be mindfulness meditation. It may help the patients inhibit cognitive processes that are preventing them from experiencing sleep.”

About this neuroscience research article

Co-authors from the University of Pittsburgh and the Medical University of South Carolina contributed to the study.

Source: Jon McBride – Brigham Young University
Publisher: Organized by NeuroscienceNews.
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
Original Research: Open access
research for “Subjective–Objective Sleep Discrepancy Is Associated With Alterations in Regional Glucose Metabolism in Patients With Insomnia and Good Sleeper Controls” by Daniel B Kay, PhD, Helmet T Karim, BS, Adriane M Soehner, PhD, Brant P Hasler, PhD, Jeffrey A James, BS, Anne Germain, PhD, Martica H Hall, PhD, Peter L Franzen, PhD, Julie C Price, PhD, Eric A Nofzinger, MD, and Daniel J Buysse, MD in Sleep. Published March 2018.
doi:10.1093/sleep/zsx155

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]Brigham Young University “Why People With Insomnia Don’t Know They’re Asleep.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 29 March 2018.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/sleep-insomnia-8707/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]Brigham Young University (2018, March 29). Why People With Insomnia Don’t Know They’re Asleep. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved March 29, 2018 from https://neurosciencenews.com/sleep-insomnia-8707/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]Brigham Young University “Why People With Insomnia Don’t Know They’re Asleep.” https://neurosciencenews.com/sleep-insomnia-8707/ (accessed March 29, 2018).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Subjective–Objective Sleep Discrepancy Is Associated With Alterations in Regional Glucose Metabolism in Patients With Insomnia and Good Sleeper Controlse

Objectives
Sleep discrepancies are common in primary insomnia (PI) and include reports of longer sleep onset latency (SOL) than measured by polysomnography (PSG) or “negative SOL discrepancy.” We hypothesized that negative SOL discrepancy in PI would be associated with higher relative glucose metabolism during nonrapid eye movement (NREM) sleep in brain networks involved in conscious awareness, including the salience, left executive control, and default mode networks.

Methods
PI (n = 32) and good sleeper controls (GS; n = 30) completed [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) scans during NREM sleep, and relative regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose (rCMRglc) was measured. Sleep discrepancy was calculated by subtracting PSG-measured SOL on the PET night from corresponding self-report values the following morning. We tested for interactions between group (PI vs. GS) and SOL discrepancy for rCMRglc during NREM sleep using both a region of interest mask and exploratory whole-brain analyses.

Results
Significant group by SOL discrepancy interactions for rCMRglc were observed in several brain regions (pcorrected < .05 for all clusters). In the PI group, more negative SOL discrepancy (self-reported > PSG-measured SOL) was associated with significantly higher relative rCMRglc in the right anterior insula and middle/posterior cingulate during NREM sleep. In GS, more positive SOL discrepancy (self-reported < PSG-measured SOL) was associated with significantly higher relative rCMRglc in the right anterior insula, left anterior cingulate cortex, and middle/posterior cingulate cortex.

Conclusions
Although preliminary, these findings suggest regions of the brain previously shown to be involved in conscious awareness, and the perception of PSG-defined states may also be involved in the phenomena of SOL discrepancy.

Feel free to share this Brigham Young University.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.