Trauma Linked to Cognitive Decline in Adults

Summary: Recent trauma suffered during adulthood has a greater impact on aspects of cognitive functioning that trauma suffered during childhood. However, experiencing the death of a parent or parental divorce during childhood was associated with an increase in cognitive decline. People who reported experiencing traumatic events showed greater overall declines in executive function and episodic memory during later life than those who had no experience of adverse events. This was especially true for those who experienced trauma during adulthood than those who experienced trauma as children.

Source: Brandeis University

Individuals who suffer trauma in child- and adulthood may experience a greater amount of cognitive decline as they age than individuals who haven’t experienced trauma, a new study found.

The research, published in June in the Journal of Traumatic Stress, also found that recent trauma suffered in adulthood has a larger impact on some aspects of cognitive functioning than trauma in childhood.

“We found that the more adverse events experienced, such as your parents’ divorce or a parent dying, the greater the cognitive decline,” said Margie Lachman, the Minnie and Harold Fierman Professor of Psychology, who co-authored the study with psychology graduate student Kristin Lynch MS ’18.

Lynch, who earned her master’s in psychology in a year in Lachman’s lab, was first author on the journal article. Her thesis explored the effect of lifetime trauma exposure on the relationship between age and religiosity

The researchers studied roughly 2,500 adults, ages 28 to 84, between 2004 and 2013. The participants were part of the Midlife Development in the U.S. (MIDUS) study, a national longitudinal study of health and well-being in adulthood.

Participants were given a list of 12 potentially traumatic events and asked if they’d experienced any, and how negatively they were affected.

The events on the list included divorce or death of a parent during childhood, emotional or physical abuse, parental alcohol or drug addiction, combat experience and losing a home to fire, flood or natural disaster. For any of these to be considered traumatic, respondents needed to indicate they caused severe emotional distress.

Subjects were also asked a series of questions that tested their cognitive abilities in two areas: executive functioning (EF) and episodic memory (EM). EF pertains to such skills as focusing attention, planning, problem-solving, and multitasking. The test of EM involved remembering recently learned information.

The scientists compared the results of individuals who said they had lived through trauma with those who indicated they hadn’t and tested their EM and EF over the course of nine years.

Those respondents who said they had experienced more traumatic events showed greater declines in both EF and EM.

Lachman said this may be because trauma has been linked to stress and depression, both of which are known to impair cognitive functioning. Trauma is also linked to metabolic disease, inflammation, and disruption of the body’s immune system, which are likewise also known to harm the brain’s performance.

Lynch stressed that suffering trauma does not automatically mean an individual will suffer greater cognitive impairment in later life. The impact of trauma varies and some people are more resilient or receive treatment that can mitigate the effects.

This is an abstract drawing of faces
Those respondents who said they had experienced more traumatic events showed greater declines in both EF and EM. Image is credited to Brandeis University.

Lynch, who is now a PhD student at the John Jay College of Criminal Justice in New York, also said the effects of cognitive decline can be subtle and may go unnoticed. “It might not feel like there’s an effect on your day-to-day functioning,” she said.

Lachman and Lynch also looked at whether childhood or more recent traumatic experiences had a greater effect on cognition. They found that individuals exposed to trauma later in life had a greater decline in EF than individuals whose first traumatic event occurred earlier in life. The amount of decline in EM did not vary based on when the event occurred in life, a finding that the researchers said required further study.

Lachman speculated there may be more of an opportunity to recover from trauma that occurs years before, in childhood. She said children may be more likely to receive supportive interventions or the increased plasticity of their brains may make them more likely to adapt in the long term.

About this psychology research article

Source:
Brandeis University
Contacts:
Lawrence Goodman – Brandeis University
Image Source:
The image is credited to Brandeis University.

Original Research: Closed access
“The Effects of Lifetime Trauma Exposure on Cognitive Functioning in Midlife” by Kristin S. Lynch, Margie E. Lachman. Journal of Traumatic Stress.


Abstract

The Effects of Lifetime Trauma Exposure on Cognitive Functioning in Midlife

Accumulating evidence suggests that lifetime trauma exposure is associated with adulthood cognitive functioning. However, the nature and extent of this relation have yet to be fully explored. We used multilevel modeling to examine trauma exposure and age at first trauma exposure as predictors of the level of and change in cognitive functioning over a 9‐year period. Data were from the Midlife in the United States study, a national survey that began in 1995. Data regarding trauma exposure and age at first exposure were obtained from the 2004 wave, whereas cognitive data were obtained from the 2004 and 2013 waves. The analyses were conducted using data from the 2,471 participants (age range: 28–84 years) who had complete data on all variables from the 2004 wave. Lifetime trauma exposure predicted change in executive functioning (EF), B = −0.03, SE = 0.01, p = .015, 95% CI [−0.05, −0.01]; and episodic memory, B = −0.05, SE = 0.02, p = .023, 95% CI [−0.10, −0.01], such that individuals with more trauma exposure had more decline over 9 years. Age at first exposure also predicted change in EF, B = −0.002, SE = 0.00, p = .009, 95% CI [−0.004, −0.001], such that individuals who were first exposed to trauma later in life had greater EF decline than individuals whose first traumatic event occurred earlier in life. Delta pseudo‐urn:x-wiley:08949867:media:jts22522:jts22522-math-0001 values were moderate, ΔpseudoR2 = .17–.39. These findings identify trauma exposure as a risk factor for cognitive decline in adulthood and highlight the elevated risk associated with adulthood trauma exposure.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.