This shows a couple at a crossroads. One way shows a bright future, the other, a darker future.
But there were some key differences in the brain activity of the socially rich group. Credit: Neuroscience News

How Upbringing Shapes Risk Decisions

Summary: A new study suggests that the way you take risks as an adult may depend less on how risky you are by nature and more on the types of resources you had growing up. Researchers found that people who were “socially rich” (strong social support, less money) and “economically rich” (more money, less social support) took similar risks but used different brain regions to get there.

fMRI scans revealed that socially rich individuals relied more on visual and attention-related brain areas during risk-taking, and their current social support influenced how much brain activity was needed. The findings highlight how early-life social and economic experiences can shape the neural strategies behind our decision-making, even when the outcomes look the same.

Key Facts

  • Different Resources, Same Risks: Socially rich and economically rich participants took similar risks but used different brain pathways.
  • Social Support Influence: In socially rich individuals, current social support reduced extra brain activity, leading to more efficient risk-taking.
  • Neural “Brake”: Activity in the supramarginal gyrus was linked to more cautious decision-making across participants.

Source: Cornell University

How risk-averse are you?

The answer might be based on how you and your brain adapted to the environment you grew up in, according to researchers in the College of Human Ecology (CHE).

They found people who are socially rich, with strong social support but whose family had less money, and those who are economically rich, having more money but less social support, take similar levels of risk but activate different parts of their brains.

This approach is novel because adversities like a lack of social support or money tend to be grouped together in studies instead of being viewed as discrete factors.

The study, published July 4 in Cerebral Cortex, was written by Minwoo Lee, a postdoctoral researcher in the College of Human Ecology (CHE). Lee is a member of the Life History Lab led by Marlen Z. Gonzalez, assistant professor of psychology in CHE and a co-author. 

“Distinct early-life environments may offer you different types of resources, and different strategies, to solve common problems in life,” said Lee.

“This, over time, may shape the specific cognitive mechanisms or contextual factors people rely on to solve similar problems as adults.”

“And it is important to include resources as part of that equation,” said Gonzalez. “There are multiple resources that can be used to overcome stressors and adversity.”

They sought to understand how people may leverage the different types of resources and challenges encountered during their youth – what they call developmental resource asymmetry – and if those resources and experiences influence choices later in life.

The study involved 43 Cornell students. While an all-student sample may not normally be ideal, the researchers said it was a strength of this study.

“That’s because you have individuals who have variability in their experiences, but end up coming together in the same place: here on campus,” said Gonzalez. “I tend to think of psychological costs and behaviors as tactics and strategies they use to navigate the world around them.”

The participants took part in a computerized risk-taking game while undergoing fMRI brain scans. In the game, they pumped virtual balloons to earn money, 5 cents per pump. At any point, they could choose to cash out.

There were three types of balloons: a risky balloon that could pop anytime, losing all collected earnings; a safe balloon that delivered a guaranteed reward when pumped to the end; and a neutral balloon that yielded no gain or loss.

The authors were particularly interested in brain activity associated with risk-taking – that is, when participants pressed buttons despite the risk of losing rewards.

Participants then completed a survey that gathered their demographic details and the resources available to them both in the past and currently, including their parents’ income during childhood, how they perceived their childhood neighborhood’s quality and safety, their current economic resources, and the level of social support they recently got from family and friends.

The survey data placed participants into two categories: socially rich and economically rich. 

Lee and Gonzalez found no differences in risk-taking tendencies between groups; however, they observed that activity in a brain region called the supramarginal gyrus increased in participants who were more cautious, suggesting it may serve as a neural “brake” on risky behavior.

But there were some key differences in the brain activity of the socially rich group. First, when they took risks, areas in the brain involved in visual and attention processing were more active for this group.

And the level of brain activity in those areas depended on their current social resources; those with more current social support had less additional brain activity, took more risks and earned more money than those in the socially rich group who had less social support.

“We’re able to show that there is some common mechanism that’s involved across both groups that helps them engage in risk-taking situations,” said Lee.

“But there seems to be this calibration process that is not necessarily about the outcome behavior itself, but how individuals get there. Even though they behaved similarly, they were relying on differential neural and contextual mechanisms to take risks.”

Insight into how social and economic experiences in childhood can shape one’s developmental calibration could help tailor support to individuals, whether through public policy, community investment or even student support services, the researchers said.

“In an ideal world, you’d have a university system that gives the economic and social support that every student needs,” said Gonzalez.

“You can emphasize the things they know how to utilize that may be missing. For example, if you have students who were socially rich, but are missing that here on campus, focus on increasing the level of social support and maybe they won’t have to work twice as hard for the same outcome.”

About this neurodevelopment and risk-taking research news

Author: Juan Vazquez-Leddon
Source: Cornell University
Contact: Juan Vazquez-Leddon – Cornell University
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Closed access.
Asymmetric access to social vs. economic resources during development calibrates socio-cognitive pathways to risk-taking in emerging adults” by Minwoo Lee et al. Cerebral Cortex


Abstract

Asymmetric access to social vs. economic resources during development calibrates socio-cognitive pathways to risk-taking in emerging adults

Developmental plasticity enables organisms to adapt to early-life environments by tailoring neurocognitive and behavioral strategies to local risks and resources.

However, existing research often reduces this dynamic calibration to aggregate exposure to adversity, leaving the effect of distinct early-life environments on adult behaviors and brain functions poorly understood.

We investigated how differential childhood socio-economic contexts influence adult risk-taking and associated mechanisms.

Forty-eight adults were recruited, and grouped based on their relative access to social (socially-rich) or economic (economically-rich) resources during childhood and completed a balloon analog risk task during functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning.

Risk-taking tendencies were estimated via computational modeling and analyzed relative to developmental and current socio-economic contexts.

While groups showed similar average risk-taking tendencies, for socially-rich participants only, greater current social support correlated with lower risk-taking. Similarly, risk-taking in both groups coincided with activation in the supramarginal gyrus.

However, socially-rich participants uniquely recruited occipito-parietal cortices during risk-taking, a pattern attenuated by higher current social support.

Across groups, supramarginal gyrus–prefrontal cortex connectivity tracked mismatches between childhood- and current resource environments, potentially reflecting “sensitized-specialization” of neural systems.

Our findings highlight how exposure to distinct early-life environments shapes divergent neurocognitive mechanisms underlying adult risk-taking, offering insights for developing context-sensitive interventions.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.