Reviving Chivalry: Small Favors Can Yield Large Returns

Many people hold the door open for strangers. But what do people give in return?

Neuroscientists at the Brain and Creativity Institute at USC have found the door holder’s effort is a key factor in whether someone returns a favor or says “thank you.”

If the door holder made a high effort by making eye contact, smiling and holding open the door, more recipients would say “thank you,” researchers found. If the engaging door holder also dropped some pens, the recipient was more likely to stop and help pick them up.

The moral of this study is: “small favors can have a sizable influence on our behavior, inspiring us to spend energy to help others, lending credence to the idea that we have a drive to ‘pay it forward,’” said USC neuroscience and psychology researcher Glenn Fox, who led the study published online Nov. 12 in Frontiers In Psychology.

“This study shows that gratitude has consequences,” said Antonio Damasio, director of the BCI and Dornsife Neuroimaging Institute at USC, and professor of psychology and neurology at USC Dornsife College of Letters, Arts and Sciences.

“It is not only the recipient of the act or gift who gains; it is also the doer or giver,” Damasio added. “When you are courteous to another person or when you offer gifts, you are doing something that is good for you. Interestingly, it can be rewarding for yourself, and it can reduce stress. It can actually be good for your health.”

Two experiments

Fox said he wanted to see how a courtesy such as door-holding results in reciprocation ranging from a “thank you” to even larger acts of repayment. To test his hypothesis, USC students involved in the study opened doors for more than 300 strangers in two experiments.

In the first experiment, door holders who made a high effort – smiling and making eye contact with the strangers they were helping – were told “thank you” more often than the door holders who behaved passively as they propped open the door with low effort, checking text messages on their cell phones. After the door-holding test, participants were asked by another experimenter to take a time-consuming survey.

Holding door open.
Ninety-seven participants of 194 thanked the door holders, regardless of their effort. Most – more than 84 percent – were thanking a door holder who made a high effort.

Of the 120 study participants, 24 thanked the door holder, regardless of their level of effort. However, most of those were in cases where door holders made a high effort, the researchers said.

“Although the participants in the high-effort condition were not more likely to take the survey than those in the low-effort, our field notes showed they were more polite and cordial when asked about the survey,” Fox said.

For the second study, researchers studied whether people will also return the favor somehow. Door holders in this experiment were toting a file box that had 12 pens on top that spilled out sometime after opening the door.

Who said “thank you,” and stopped and helped? Again, researchers found the response depended on the door holder’s effort.

Ninety-seven participants of 194 thanked the door holders, regardless of their effort. Most – more than 84 percent – were thanking a door holder who made a high effort.

Fifty-four of the 194 participants helped the door holders pick up their pens. Most participants – 64 percent – were helping door holders who had made a high effort, versus 19 percent who assisted door holders who had made a low effort.

Don’t expect a ‘thank you’

The findings were two-fold: The study shows a small favor can inspire reciprocal acts. At the same time, researchers recognized that people do not feel obligated to say “thank you” or help, even when they have received a favor.


Empty Thanks

“We see for the first time that verbal thanking and reciprocal helping are not inherently correlated,” the researchers noted.

The study raises several other questions. Researchers said future studies should examine how eye contact, the type of favor and other such factors may influence recipients’ responses.

About this psychology research

Source: Emily Gersema – USC
Image Source: The image is adapted from the USC video
Video Source: The video is available at the USC YouTube page
Original Research: Full open access research for “How Does the Effort Spent to Hold a Door Affect Verbal Thanks and Reciprocal Help?” by Glenn R. Fox, Helder Filipe Araujo, Michael J. Metke, Chris Shafer and Antonio Damasio in Frontiers In Psychology. Published online November 12 2015 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01737


Abstract

How Does the Effort Spent to Hold a Door Affect Verbal Thanks and Reciprocal Help?

When someone holds a door for us we often respond with a verbal “thanks.” But given such a trivial favor, our feelings can vary considerably depending on how the door is held. Studies have shown that verbal thanking increases in relation to door-holding effort. However, it is unclear how such a favor can lead to verbal thanks in addition to reciprocal help. We examined how holding a door in an effortful or non-effortful manner relates to verbal thanking and reciprocal helping. We measured: (1) whether participants verbally thanked the experimenter, (2) whether they agreed to help another person by taking a survey, and (3) whether they helped pick up objects (pens) that the door-holder subsequently dropped. Participants in the effortful condition were more likely to offer verbal thanks, to help pick up the pens, and to walk a greater distance to pick them up. Participants who thanked the door-holder, however, were not more likely to provide help.

“How Does the Effort Spent to Hold a Door Affect Verbal Thanks and Reciprocal Help?” by Glenn R. Fox, Helder Filipe Araujo, Michael J. Metke, Chris Shafer and Antonio Damasio in Frontiers In Psychology. Published online November 12 2015 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01737

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.