Hacking Memory to Follow Through with Intentions

Summary: Linking simple, often forgotten tasks to distinctive cues may help us to remember to follow through, a new study reports.

Source: APS.

Whether it’s paying the electric bill or taking the clothes out of the dryer, there are many daily tasks that we fully intend to complete and then promptly forget about. New research suggests that linking these tasks to distinctive cues that we’ll encounter at the right place and the right time may help us remember to follow through. The research is published in Psychological Science.

There are many ways we can try to remind ourselves to do something in the future – we can set a calendar alert, jot down a quick note, or even use the old-fashioned string-around-the-finger trick. But the problem with many of these strategies is that they don’t provide a reminder that will be noticed when we need it most.

“Our results suggest that people are more likely to follow through on their good intentions if they are reminded to follow through by noticeable cues that appear at the exact place and time in which follow-through can occur,” says study author and psychological scientist Todd Rogers of Harvard Kennedy School.

Rogers and co-author Katherine Milkman of the Wharton School at the University of Pennsylvania hypothesized that “reminders through association” may be a tool for remembering and following through. By design, these cue-based reminders don’t depend on any technology other than the human mind and they are delivered exactly when we need them.

In one study, 87 participants completed an hour-long computer task. During the task, they learned that they would be able to have $1 donated to a food bank in addition to receiving compensation for participating. To ensure the donation would be made, however, they would have to pick up a paper clip when they collected their payment.
Some students saw a second message stating that an elephant statue would be sitting on the counter where they collected their payment as a reminder to pick up a paper clip; others were simply thanked for their participation.

This simple cue effectively boosted follow-through. The results showed that 74% of the students who received the elephant statue as a cue ended up taking a paper clip compared to only 42% of those who didn’t receive a cue.

But this doesn’t mean that any cue will do; additional studies conducted online indicated that distinctiveness matters – a reminder cannot work if it is not noticed.

Again, participants learned about an opportunity to support a charitable organization. To have a donation made on their behalf, they would have to choose a specific answer on a specific page of the survey they were about to complete. On that page, they would see a cue to remind them to select the correct answer.

The results revealed that cues that were distinctive relative to the other cues in the surrounding environment were more effective reminders. In one experiment, for example, Rogers and Milkman found that a distinctive cue – an image of one of the aliens from the Toy Story movies – was more effective than a written reminder when both cues were surrounded by other flyers and promotional signs.

Data collected from customers at a coffee shop suggest that the “reminders through association” approach may also be useful for organizations that want to help their clients remember to follow through on intentions. Over the course of one business day, 500 customers were given a coupon that would be valid at the coffee shop two days later. Only some customers were told that a stuffed alien would be sitting near the cash register to remind them to use their coupon. About 24% of the customers who were given a cue remembered to use their coupon compared to only 17% of the customers who received no cue – a 40% increase in coupon usage.

While distinctive cues may serve as helpful reminders, people don’t always make the most of them. Results from another online study with 605 participants showed that participants failed to anticipate the liAPSations of their own memories – in doing so, they missed out on potential earnings by choosing not to pay a nominal fee to receive a cue-based reminder.

Image shows a post=it note and keyboard.
In one study, 87 participants completed an hour-long computer task. During the task, they learned that they would be able to have $1 donated to a food bank in addition to receiving compensation for participating. To ensure the donation would be made, however, they would have to pick up a paper clip when they collected their payment. NeuroscienceNews.com image is adapted from the APS press release.

Taken together, the findings suggest that reminders through association offer a no-cost, low-effort strategy for remembering to complete the tasks that tend to fall through the cracks in daily life.

Rogers and Milkman hope to build on this research to explore whether reminders through association might also be useful for boosting adherence to medical and other health-related regimens.

About this memory research article

Funding: This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health’s National Institute on Aging through the Roybal Center for Behavior Change in Health and Savings at the National Bureau of Economic Research (NBER) under Award No.P30AG034532. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NBER.

Source: Anna Mikulak – APS
Image Source: This NeuroscienceNews.com image is adapted from the APS press release.
Original Research: Abstract for “Reminders Through Association” by Todd Rogers and Katherine L. Milkman in Psychological Science. Published online May 20 2016 doi:10.1177/0956797616643071

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]APS. “Hacking Memory to Follow Through with Intentions.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 23 May 2016.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/tasks-memory-cues-psychology-4279/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]APS. (2016, May 23). Hacking Memory to Follow Through with Intentions. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved May 23, 2016 from https://neurosciencenews.com/tasks-memory-cues-psychology-4279/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]APS. “Hacking Memory to Follow Through with Intentions.” https://neurosciencenews.com/tasks-memory-cues-psychology-4279/ (accessed May 23, 2016).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Reminders Through Association

People often fail to follow through on good intentions. While limited self-control is frequently the culprit, another cause is simply forgetting to enact intentions when opportunities arise. We introduce a novel, potent approach to facilitating follow-through: the reminders-through-association approach. This approach involves associating intentions (e.g., to mail a letter on your desk tomorrow) with distinctive cues that will capture attention when you have opportunities to act on those intentions (e.g., Valentine’s Day flowers that arrived late yesterday, which are sitting on your desk). We showed that cue-based reminders are more potent when the cues they employ are distinctive relative to (a) other regularly encountered stimuli and (b) other stimuli encountered concurrently. Further, they can be more effective than written or electronic reminder messages, and they are undervalued and underused. The reminders-through-association approach, developed by integrating and expanding on past research on self-control, reminders, and prospective memory, can be a powerful tool for policymakers and individuals.

“Reminders Through Association” by Todd Rogers and Katherine L. Milkman in Psychological Science. Published online May 20 2016 doi:10.1177/0956797616643071

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.