The Brain at Work: Spotting Half Hidden Objects

Summary: Researchers have identified how signals from the visual and prefrontal cortices may interact to help us recognize shapes that are not entirely visible.

Source: UW Medicine.

How does a driver’s brain realize that a stop sign is behind a bush when only a red edge is showing? Or how can a monkey suspect that the yellow sliver in the leaves is a round piece of fruit?

The human (and non-human) primate brain is remarkable in recognizing objects when the view is nearly blocked. This skill let our ancient ancestors find food and avoid danger. It continues to be critical to making sense of our surroundings.

UW Medicine scientists are conducting research to discover ways that the brain operates when figuring out shapes, from those that are completely visible to those that are mostly hidden.

Although computers can beat the world’s best chess players, scientists have not yet designed artificial intelligence that performs as well as the average person in distinguishing shapes that are semi-obscured.

Studies of signals generated by the brain are helping to fill in the picture of what goes on when looking at, then trying to recognize, shapes. Such research is also showing why attempts have failed to mechanically replicate the ability of humans and primates to identify partially hidden objects.

The most recent results of this work are published Sept. 19 in the scientific journal eLife.

The senior investigator is Anitha Pasupathy, associate professor of biological structure at the University of Washington School of Medicine in Seattle and a member of the Washington National Primate Research Center.

There’s a computer game at the center that can be played to tell if two shapes are alike or different. A correct answer wins a treat. As dots start to appear over the shapes, the task becomes more difficult.

The researchers learned that, during the simpler part of the game, the brain generates signals in certain areas of the visual cortex – the part for sight. The neurons, or brain nerve cells, in that section respond more strongly to uncovered shapes.

However, when the shapes begin to disappear behind the dots, certain neurons in the part of the brain that governs functions like memory and planning – the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex – respond more intensely.

The researchers also observed that many of the neurons in the visual cortex had two quick response peaks. The second one occurred after the response onset in the thinking section of the brain. This seemed to enhance the response of the neurons in the visual cortex to the partially hidden shapes.

The results, according to Pasupathy, suggest how signals from the two different areas of the brain — thinking and vision — could interact to assist in recognizing shapes that are not fully visible.

They researches believe that other regions of the brain, in addition to those they studied, are likely to participate in object recognition.

“It’s not just the information flowing from the eyes into the sensory location of the brain that’s important to know what a shape is when it’s partially covered,” she said. “Feedback from other regions of the brain also help in making this determination.”

Relying only on the image of an object that appears on the eye’s retina makes it hard to make out what it is, because that image could have many interpretations.

Image shows dots.
This image shows how the difficulty in recognizing the shape of an object increases the more it is covered with dots. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Pasupathy ShapeLAB/UW Medicine.

Recognition stems not only from the physical appearance of the object, but also the scene, the context, the degree of covering, and the viewer’s experience, the researchers explained.

The study helps advance knowledge about how the brain typically works in solving this frequently encountered perceptual puzzle.

“The neural mechanisms that mediate perceptual capacities, such as this one, have been largely unknown, which is why we were interested in studying them,” Pasupathy noted.

Their recent findings also make the scientists wonder if impairments in this and other types of communication between the cognitive and sensory parts of the brain might have a role in certain difficulties that people with autism or Alzheimer’s encounter.

Pasupathy said, for example, some people with autism have a profound inability to function in cluttered or disorderly environments. They have problems processing sensory information and can become confused and distressed. Many patients with Alzheimer’s disease experience what is called visual agnosia. They have no trouble seeing objects, but they can’t tell what they are.

“So understanding how the sensory and cognitive areas in the brain communicate is of utmost importance to ultimately understand what might go wrong inside the nervous system that can cause these deficits,” Pasupathy said.

About this neuroscience research article

Other researchers on this study were Amber Fyall, Yasmine El-Shamayleh, Hannah Choi, and Eric Shea-Brown, all from the University of Washington in Seattle.

Funding: NIH/National Eye Institute, Washington National Primate Research Center, National Science Foundation, Washington Research Foundation funded this study.

Source: Leila Gray – UW Medicine
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Pasupathy ShapeLAB/UW Medicine.
Original Research: Full open access research for “Dynamic representation of partially occluded objects in primate prefrontal and visual cortex” by Amber M Fyall, Yasmine El-Shamayleh, Hannah Choi, Eric Shea-Brown, and Anitha Pasupathy in eLife. Published online September 19 2017 doi:10.7554/eLife.25784.001

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]UW Medicine “The Brain at Work: Spotting Half Hidden Objects.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 19 September 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/observation-hidden-objects-7520/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]UW Medicine (2017, September 19). The Brain at Work: Spotting Half Hidden Objects. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved September 19, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/observation-hidden-objects-7520/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]UW Medicine “The Brain at Work: Spotting Half Hidden Objects.” https://neurosciencenews.com/observation-hidden-objects-7520/ (accessed September 19, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Dynamic representation of partially occluded objects in primate prefrontal and visual cortex

Successful recognition of partially occluded objects is presumed to involve dynamic interactions between brain areas responsible for vision and cognition, but neurophysiological evidence for the involvement of feedback signals is lacking. Here, we demonstrate that neurons in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) of monkeys performing a shape discrimination task respond more strongly to occluded than unoccluded stimuli. In contrast, neurons in visual area V4 respond more strongly to unoccluded stimuli. Analyses of V4 response dynamics reveal that many neurons exhibit two transient response peaks, the second of which emerges after vlPFC response onset and displays stronger selectivity for occluded shapes. We replicate these findings using a model of V4/vlPFC interactions in which occlusion-sensitive vlPFC neurons feed back to shape-selective V4 neurons, thereby enhancing V4 responses and selectivity to occluded shapes. These results reveal how signals from frontal and visual cortex could interact to facilitate object recognition under occlusion.

“Dynamic representation of partially occluded objects in primate prefrontal and visual cortex” by Amber M Fyall, Yasmine El-Shamayleh, Hannah Choi, Eric Shea-Brown, and Anitha Pasupathy in eLife. Published online September 19 2017 doi:10.7554/eLife.25784.001

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.