Brain’s ‘Memory Center’ Needed to Recognize Image Sequences but Not Single Sights

Summary: While the visual cortex stores individual images in the brain, the hippocampus plays a significant role in the ability to recognize a sequence of images.

Source: Picower Institute

A new MIT study of how a mammalian brain remembers what it sees shows that while individual images are stored in the visual cortex, the ability to recognize a sequence of sights critically depends on guidance from the hippocampus, a deeper structure strongly associated with memory but shrouded in mystery about exactly how.

By suggesting that the hippocampus isn’t needed for basic storage of images so much as identifying the chronological relationship they may have, the new research published in Current Biology can bring neuroscientists closer to understanding how the brain coordinates long-term visual memory across key regions.

“This offers the opportunity to actually understand, in a very concrete way, how the hippocampus contributes to memory storage in the cortex,” said senior author Mark Bear, Picower Professor of Neuroscience in The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and MIT’s Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences.

Essentially, the hippocampus acts to influence how images are stored in the cortex if they have a sequential relationship, said lead author Peter Finnie, a former postdoc in Bear’s lab.

“The exciting part of this is that the visual cortex seems to be involved in encoding both very simple visual stimuli and also temporal sequences of them, and yet the hippocampus is selectively involved in how that sequence is stored,” Finnie said.

To have hippocampus and have not

To make their findings, the researchers including former postdoc Rob Komorowski trained mice with two forms of visual recognition memory discovered in Bear’s lab.

The first form of memory, called stimulus selective response plasticity (SRP) involves learning to recognize a non-rewarding, non-threatening single visual stimulus after it has been presented over and over. As learning occurs, visual cortex neurons produce an increasingly strong electrical response and the mouse ceases paying attention to the once novel, but now decidedly uninteresting, image.

The second form of memory, visual sequence plasticity, involves learning to recognize and predict a sequence of images. Here, too, the once novel but now familiar and innocuous sequence comes to evoke an elevated electrical response, and it is much greater than what is observed if the same stimuli are presented in reverse order or at a different speed.

In prior studies Bear’s lab has shown that the images in each form of memory are stored in the visual cortex, and are even specific to which eye beheld them if only one did.

But the researchers were curious about whether and how the hippocampus might contribute to these forms of memory and cortical plasticity. After all, like some other forms of memory that depend on the hippocampus, SRP only takes hold after a period of “consolidation,” for instance overnight during sleep. To test whether there is a role for hippocampus, they chemically removed large portions of the structure in a group of mice and looked for differences between groups in the tell-tale electrical response each kind of recognition memory should evoke.

This shows two brain slices. One has a hippocampus, the other does not
A pair of mouse brain cross-sections shows an unaltered one on the upper left and one with significant removal of the hippocampus on the lower right. Credit: Peter FInnie/MIT Picower Institute

Mice with or without a hippocampus performed equally well in learning SRP (not only measured electrophysiologically but also behaviorally), suggesting that the hippocampus was not needed for that form of memory. It appears to arise, and even consolidate, entirely within the visual cortex.

Visual sequence plasticity, however, did not occur without an intact hippocampus, the researchers found. Mice without the structure showed no elevated electrical response to the sequences when tested, no ability to recognize them in reverse or when delayed and no inclination to “fill in the blank” when one was missing. It was as if the visual sequence –and even each image in the sequence–was not familiar.

“Together these findings are consistent with a specific role for the hippocampus in predictive response generation during exposure to familiar temporal patterns of visual stimulation,” the authors wrote.

New finding from a classic approach

The experiments follow in a long tradition of attempting to understand the hippocampus by assessing what happens when it’s damaged. For decades, neuroscientists at MIT and elsewhere were able to learn from a man known as “H.M.,” who had undergone hippocampal removal to relieve epileptic seizures. His memory of his past before the surgery remained intact but he exhibited an inability to form “declarative” memories of new experiences such as meeting someone, or performing an activity.

Over time, however, scientists realized that he could be trained to learn motor tasks better, even though he wouldn’t remember the training itself. The experiments helped to reveal that for many different forms of memory there is a “division of labor” among regions of the brain that may or may not include the hippocampus.

The new study, Bear and Finnie said, produces a clear distinction through the division of labor in visual memory between simple recognition of images and the more complex task of recognizing of sequence structure.

“It’s a nice dividing line,” Bear said. “It’s the same region of the brain, the same method of an animal looking at images on a screen. All we are changing is the temporal structure of the stimulus.”

Alzheimer’s assessment?

Previous research in the lab showed that SRP and visual sequence plasticity arise via different molecular mechanisms. SRP can be disrupted by blocking receptors for the neurotransmitter glutamate on involved neurons while sequence plasticity depends on receptors for acetylcholine.

The next question Bear wants to address, therefore, is whether an acetylcholine producing circuit links the hippocampus to the visual cortex to accomplish sequence learning. Neurons that release acetylcholine in the cortex happen to be among the earliest disrupted in Alzheimer’s disease.

If the circuit for sequence learning indeed runs through those neurons, Bear speculated, then assessing people for differences in SRP and sequence learning could become a way to diagnose early onset of dementia progression.

Funding: The National Eye Institute of the National Institutes of Health and the JPB Foundation funded the research.

About this visual neuroscience research news

Source: Picower Institute
Contact: David Orenstein – Picower Institute
Image: The image is credited to Peter FInnie/MIT Picower Institute

Original Research: Open access.
The spatiotemporal organization of experience dictates hippocampal involvement in primary visual cortical plasticity” by Mark Bear et al. Current Biology


Abstract

The spatiotemporal organization of experience dictates hippocampal involvement in primary visual cortical plasticity

Highlights

  • Hippocampus is required for long-term plasticity in V1 elicited by visual sequences
  • Hippocampal damage impairs anticipatory activity when sequence elements are withheld
  • Hippocampus is required for V1 stimulus representations shaped by temporal context

Summary

The hippocampus and neocortex are theorized to be crucial partners in the formation of long-term memories.

Here, we assess hippocampal involvement in two related forms of experience-dependent plasticity in the primary visual cortex (V1) of mice. Like control animals, those with hippocampal lesions exhibit potentiation of visually evoked potentials after passive daily exposure to a phase-reversing oriented grating stimulus, which is accompanied by long-term habituation of a reflexive behavioral response. Thus, low-level recognition memory is formed independently of the hippocampus.

However, response potentiation resulting from daily exposure to a fixed sequence of four oriented gratings is severely impaired in mice with hippocampal damage. A feature of sequence plasticity in V1 of controls, which is absent in lesioned mice, is the generation of predictive responses to an anticipated stimulus element when it is withheld or delayed.

Thus, the hippocampus is involved in encoding temporally structured experience, even within the primary sensory cortex.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.