Chronic Fatigue Patients More Likely to Suppress Emotions

Summary: According to researchers, people with chronic fatigue may be more likely to suppress their emotions.

Source: APA.

“Fight or flight” biological response may contribute to fatigue.

Chronic fatigue syndrome patients report they are more anxious and distressed than people who don’t have the condition, and they are also more likely to suppress those emotions. In addition, when under stress, they show greater activation of the biological ”fight or flight” mechanism, which may add to their fatigue, according to new research published by the American Psychological Association.

“We hope that this research will contribute to a greater understanding of the needs of people with chronic fatigue syndrome, some of whom may tend not to communicate their experiences of symptoms or stress to other people,” said the study’s lead author, Katharine Rimes, PhD, of King’s College London. “Others may be unaware of the difficulties experienced by chronic fatigue syndrome patients and therefore not provide appropriate support.”

Participants who felt that expressing their emotions was socially unacceptable were more likely to suppress them. This was the case for both chronic fatigue patients and healthy people, according to the study published in the APA journal Health Psychology.

This study of 160 people in the U.K. relied on self and observer reports, as well as physiological responses that were collected before, during or after the participants watched a distressing film clip. Half of the participants had been diagnosed with chronic fatigue syndrome while the rest were healthy.

Half of each group were instructed to suppress their emotions and half were told to express their feelings as they wished. Their reactions were filmed and rated by independent observers. Skin conductance was measured because this increases with greater sweating, which is a sign of activation of the body’s sympathetic nervous system. This is often known as the biological fight or flight system used to cope with stress.

Regardless of the instruction they received, the chronic fatigue syndrome participants reported higher anxiety and sadness, and their skin responses indicated they were more distressed than the healthy control group, both before and after the film. However, those emotions in the chronic fatigue group were less likely to be picked up by the independent observers.

Greater activation of the fight or flight system was associated with greater increases in fatigue in the people with chronic fatigue syndrome, but not among healthy people. “Patients with chronic fatigue syndrome often tell us that stress worsens their symptoms, but this study demonstrates a possible biological mechanism underlying this effect,” said Rimes.

Image shows a tired looking man.
Regardless of the instruction they received, the chronic fatigue syndrome participants reported higher anxiety and sadness, and their skin responses indicated they were more distressed than the healthy control group, both before and after the film. However, those emotions in the chronic fatigue group were less likely to be picked up by the independent observers. NeuroscienceNews.com image is for illustrative purposes only.

The authors note that this study was conducted with mainly white patients who were attending a special clinic for chronic fatigue syndrome patients and that more research is needed to determine whether elevated emotional suppression would also be found in chronic fatigue patients in more diverse populations.

Since this study was conducted among people who had already been diagnosed with chronic fatigue syndrome, this does not indicate a causal link between emotional suppression and the syndrome itself, Rimes added.

“These findings may help us understand why some chronic fatigue syndrome patients don’t seek out social support at times of stress,” said Rimes. “Patients’ families may benefit from information about how to best support patients who tend to hide their emotions.”

About this psychology research article

Funding: Funding information is not available.

Source: Audrey Hamilton – APA
Image Source: The NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
Original Research: Full open access research for “Emotional Suppression in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: Experimental Study” by Katharine A. Rimes, Joanna Ashcroft, Lauren Bryan, and Trudie Chalder in Health Psychology. Published online May 16 2016 doi:10.1037/hea0000341

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]APA. “Chronic Fatigue Patients More Likely to Suppress Emotions.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 17 May 2016.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/emotion-chronic-fatigue-4248/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]APA. (2016, May 17). Chronic Fatigue Patients More Likely to Suppress Emotions. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved May 17, 2016 from https://neurosciencenews.com/emotion-chronic-fatigue-4248/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]APA. “Chronic Fatigue Patients More Likely to Suppress Emotions.” https://neurosciencenews.com/emotion-chronic-fatigue-4248/ (accessed May 17, 2016).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Emotional Suppression in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: Experimental Study

Objective: Emotional processing differences in chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) have been reported but have rarely been investigated experimentally. This study used self-report, observer ratings, and electrodermal responses to test hypotheses about emotion suppression and autonomic reactivity.

Methods: Eighty adults with CFS and 80 healthy controls (HC) watched a distressing film clip. Half of each group were instructed to suppress their emotions and half were told to express their feelings as they wished. Their reactions were filmed and rated by independent observers. Electrodermal activity (skin conductance response) was used as a measure of sympathetic nervous system arousal.

Results: CFS participants reported higher anxiety and sadness than the HC, both before and after the film. However, observers rated the CFS group as having lower emotional expression than HC in both emotional suppression and expression choice conditions. Beliefs about the unacceptability of negative emotions were associated with greater self-reported suppression. Electrodermal responses were greater in the CFS group than HC participants. Higher skin conductance responses were associated with larger posttask increases in fatigue in the CFS participants but not in the HC.

Conclusions: CFS participants had lower observer-rated emotional expression than HC, despite greater distress and higher autonomic arousal. This may have implications for their ability to access social support at times of stress. As the degree of autonomic arousal was associated with short-term increases in fatigue in the CFS participants, this requires further investigation as a contributory factor for this condition.

“Emotional Suppression in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: Experimental Study” by Katharine A. Rimes, Joanna Ashcroft, Lauren Bryan, and Trudie Chalder in Health Psychology. Published online May 16 2016 doi:10.1037/hea0000341

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. I”M confused. I thought that there was a tendency of people with CFS to be overly emotional, which is the opposite of this study’s findings. Could it be that people with CFS, when they do suppress their emotions (repression) do so for fear of the “psychopathology” label that is often applied when they are being “overly emotional” (whatever that is).

    These folks cannot get a break. First, the Pace study’s bogus findings, now this. I would encourage any scientist who does not believe CFS is legitimate to either read all of the literature, not just the part that confirms his or her own biases, or simply switch to a different line of research.

  2. FGS: The Harvard research that gave us “fight or flight” was a flawed study that was more recently corrected. The original study used only male subjects. Hence, “fight or flight: is a male response. The female response to the same stimuli is “tend and befriend,” tend to the children, befriend other females. Same cognitive systems, presumably, but different subsequent behavior patterns.

Comments are closed.