Brain’s ‘GPS system’ toggles between present and possible future paths in real time

Summary: Study shows how hippocampal cells can represent different hypothetical scenarios consistently and systematically over time. The findings shed new light on how place cells assist in decision making and imagination.

Source: UCSF

Survival often depends on animals’ ability to make split-second decisions that rely on imagining alternative futures: If I’m being chased by a hungry predator, do I zag left to get home safely or zig right to lead the predator away from my family? When two paths diverge in a yellow wood, which will lead me to breakfast and which will lead me to become breakfast? Both look really about the same, but imagination makes all the difference.

In a study of rats navigating a simple maze, neuroscientists at UC San Francisco have discovered how the brain may generate such imagined future scenarios. The work provides a new grounding for understanding not only how the brain makes decisions but also how imagination works more broadly, the researchers say.

“One of the brain’s most amazing abilities is to imagine things that aren’t right in front of it,” said Loren Frank, PhD, a professor of physiology and Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigator in the UCSF Center for Integrative Neuroscience, co-director of the UCSF Kavli Institute for Fundamental Neuroscience, and member of the UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences. “Imagination is fundamental to decision-making, but so far neuroscience hasn’t given a good explanation of how the brain generates imagined futures in real time to inform various kinds of everyday decisions — while keeping track of reality at the same time.”

In the new study, published January 30, 2020, in Cell, Frank’s team had rats explore an M-shaped maze, while recording the firing of neurons in the hippocampus called “place cells”, which are traditionally thought to keep track of an animal’s location — like a neural GPS system. But as the rats approached a fork in the maze, the researchers discovered that their place-cell activity began to switch back and forth extremely rapidly — at a rate of eight times per second — between representing the animal’s current position and its two alternative future paths, as if to say: “Here I am — go left? — here I am — go right?”

The team also extended this finding to another type of imagined scenario. Apart from location, place cells have also been known to keep track of an animal’s travel direction. The team found that place cells representing opposite travel directions could also switch back and forth extremely rapidly, as if to say, “I’m going this way, but I could also turn around and go the other way.”

“The cells’ fast switching between present and possible paths was unmistakable because it was so regular,” said Kenneth Kay, PhD, a post-doctoral researcher at Columbia University who led the study as a graduate student in Frank’s lab. “It was exciting to see because speed plus consistency is exactly what’s needed in any number of real-world settings, for both animals and humans.”

The place cells’ oscillations between the present and possible futures didn’t appear to be directly controlling rats’ decisions about which path to choose, but did become stronger as the rats approached the decision point, Kay and colleagues found. This suggested to the researchers that the role of the hippocampus in decision-making might be to generate a “menu” of imagined scenarios for other parts of the brain that can associate these options with past experience of their value or potential danger, then make an appropriate decision based on the animal’s current drives — hungry or thirsty, fearful or bold.

“We think this shows that the hippocampus is not just responsible for the recording the past and processing the present, but for imagining the future as well,” Frank said. “This study is just a first step, but it opens new avenues for us to study how imagined scenarios are generated and evaluated in the brain as animals make decisions.”

Study Suggests New Conception of Hippocampus as Source of Imagination

The hippocampus, a seahorse-shaped structure found on each side of the brain deep in the temporal lobes, is among the most intensively studied parts of the brain.

This is a drawing of the hippocampus
“We think this shows that the hippocampus is not just responsible for the recording the past and processing the present, but for imagining the future as well,” Frank said. Image is in the public domain.

Hippocampal damage — whether by brain injury or in a disease such as Alzheimer’s — robs people of the ability to form new memories, leading 20th century scientists to describe the hippocampus as the brain’s memory center. In the 1970s, scientists identified hippocampal place cells, which spontaneously create maps of new environments as animals explore them, then store these maps for later use. This discovery, which was awarded the 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine, prompted scientists to recognize that the hippocampus is also a navigation center — responsible for, say, allowing an animal to find its way back to the place where it remembers eating those delicious blackberries last summer.

Along these lines, previous work by Frank and others has shown that place cell activity can replay an animal’s recent movements or even anticipate where an animal may be headed next, but such activity had only been seen intermittently — typically when animals were resting or pausing during ongoing movement — as actively considering their next move.

The new Cell study is the first to show how hippocampal cells can represent different hypothetical scenarios consistently and systematically over time. Such a system could allow animals on the move to make extremely rapid decisions in the moment based on these imagined alternatives while also keeping track of the animal’s present reality, the researchers say. It could even play a role in the brain’s ability to generate hypothetical scenarios or thoughts more broadly.

“The regular switching between present and possible — or actual and imagined — looks like be a robust system for generating lots of ideas, not just for mechanically remembering or predicting,” Kay said. “The hippocampus could be at the root of our ability to imagine.”

Authors: Additional authors on the study were Jason Chung, Marielena Sosa, Jonathan S. Schor, Mattias Karlsson, Margaret Larkin, and Daniel Liu of UCSF.

Funding: The research was supported by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, the Simons Collaboration for the Global Brain (521921, 542981), the National Institutes of Health (R01 MH090188, R01 MH105174), the National Science Foundation (NSF) NeuroNex Award (DBI-1707398), and the Gatsby Charitable Foundation.

The authors declare no competing interests.

About this neuroscience research article

Source:
UCSF
Media Contacts:
Nicholas Weiler – UCSF
Image Source:
The image is in the public domain.

Original Research: Closed access
“Constant Sub-second Cycling between Representations of Possible Futures in the Hippocampus”. Loren Frank et al.
Cell doi:10.1016/j.cell.2020.01.014.

Abstract

Constant Sub-second Cycling between Representations of Possible Futures in the Hippocampus

Highlights
• Firing across hippocampal neurons can regularly “take turns” (cycle) every ∼125 ms
• Cycle firing is seen at single-cell, cell-pair, and population levels
• Cycle firing encodes hypothetical experience, including multiple possible futures
• Cycle coding generalizes across representational correlates, implying common process

Summary
Cognitive faculties such as imagination, planning, and decision-making entail the ability to represent hypothetical experience. Crucially, animal behavior in natural settings implies that the brain can represent hypothetical future experience not only quickly but also constantly over time, as external events continually unfold. To determine how this is possible, we recorded neural activity in the hippocampus of rats navigating a maze with multiple spatial paths. We found neural activity encoding two possible future scenarios (two upcoming maze paths) in constant alternation at 8 Hz: one scenario per ∼125-ms cycle. Further, we found that the underlying dynamics of cycling (both inter- and intra-cycle dynamics) generalized across qualitatively different representational correlates (location and direction). Notably, cycling occurred across moving behaviors, including during running. These findings identify a general dynamic process capable of quickly and continually representing hypothetical experience, including that of multiple possible futures.

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. By my different theoretical approach, imagination is product of a process of feed-back between places cells and the brain´s systemic identity. For to know the functions and activities of the brain is good to have at hands the template creator of its anatomy, which is the universal Matrix/DNA spherical diagram for all natural systems.

    In this formula we see and understand how a system has information of present, past and future, but only for its internal structure. The fact that the intense signals occurs in cells place in the hippocampus is exactly the point where the formula keeps these information, so, it was predicted by the formula. This new evolutionary jump, going to external world, must be self-projection of this old property. For those with curiosity, I wrote an article about this issue with more information, at https://theuniversalmatrix.com/en-us/articles/?p=1411

Comments are closed.