Babies can learn link between language and ethnicity

Summary: By eleven months, infants hold language dependent expectations of a speaker’s ethnicity. The study suggests babies make connections between languages based on the individuals they encounter in their environments.

Source: University of British Columbia

Eleven-month-old infants can learn to associate the language they hear with ethnicity, recent research from the University of British Columbia suggests.

The study, published April 22 by Developmental Psychobiology, found that 11-month-old infants looked more at the faces of people of Asian descent versus those of Caucasian descent when hearing Cantonese versus English–but not when hearing Spanish.

“Our findings suggest that by 11 months, infants are making connections between languages and ethnicities based on the individuals they encounter in their environments. In learning about language, infants are doing more than picking up sounds and sentences–they also learn about the speakers of language,” said Lillian May, a psychology lecturer at UBC who was lead author of the study.

The research was done in Vancouver, where approximately nine per cent of the population can speak Cantonese.

The researchers played English-learning infants of Caucasian ancestry sentences in both English and Cantonese and showed them pictures of people of Caucasian descent, and of Asian descent. When the infants heard Cantonese, they looked more at the Asian faces than when they were hearing English. When they heard English, they looked equally to Asian and Caucasian faces.

“This indicates that they have already learned that in Vancouver, both Caucasians and Asians are likely to speak English, but only Asians are likely to speak Cantonese,” noted UBC psychology professor Janet Werker, the study’s senior author.

This shows two gorgeous baby girls
Babies as young as 11 months old can associate language with ethnicity, UBC research suggests. The image is credited to Sav Nijeboer / Dr. Janet Werker’s Infant Studies Centre.Babies as young as 11 months old can associate language with ethnicity, UBC research suggests. The image is credited to Sav Nijeboer / Dr. Janet Werker’s Infant Studies Centre.

The researchers showed the same pictures to the infants while playing Spanish, to see whether they were inclined to associate any unfamiliar language with any unfamiliar ethnicity. However, in that test the infants looked equally to Asian and Caucasian faces. This suggests young infants pick up on specific language-ethnicity pairings based on the faces and languages they encounter.

“Babies are learning so much about language–even about its social use–long before they produce the first word,” said Werker. “The link between speaker characteristics and language is something no one has to teach babies. They learn it all on their own.”

The researchers are now probing how babies’ ability to link language and ethnicity might help them with language acquisition.

About this neuroscience research article

Source:
University of British Columbia
Media Contacts:
Erik Rolfsen – University of British Columbia
Image Source:
The image is credited to Sav Nijeboer / Dr. Janet Werker’s Infant Studies Centre.

Original Research: Closed access
“Who can speak that language? Eleven‐month‐old infants have language‐dependent expectations regarding speaker ethnicity”. Lillian May Andrew S. Baron Janet F. Werker.
Developmental Psychobiology. doi:10.1002/dev.21851

Abstract

Who can speak that language? Eleven‐month‐old infants have language‐dependent expectations regarding speaker ethnicity

Research demonstrates that young infants attend to the indexical characteristics of speakers, including age, gender, and ethnicity, and that the relationship between language and ethnicity is intuitive among older children. However, little research has examined whether infants, within the first year, are sensitive to the co‐occurrences of ethnicity and language. In this paper, we demonstrate that by 11 months of age, infants hold language‐dependent expectations regarding speaker ethnicity. Specifically, 11‐month‐old English‐learning Caucasian infants looked more to Asian versus Caucasian faces when hearing Cantonese versus English (Studies 1 and 3), but did not look more to Asian versus Caucasian faces when paired with Spanish (Study 2), making it unlikely that they held a general expectation that unfamiliar languages pair with unfamiliar faces. Moreover, infants who had regular exposure to one or more significant non‐Caucasian individuals showed this pattern more strongly (Study 3). Given that infants tested were raised in a multilingual metropolitan area—which includes a Caucasian population speaking many languages, but seldom Cantonese, as well as a sizeable Asian population speaking both Cantonese and English—these results are most parsimoniously explained by infants having learned specific language–ethnicity associations based on those individuals they encountered in their environment.

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.