Did Standing Up Change Our Brains?

Although lots of animals are smart, humans are even smarter. How and why do we think and act so differently from other species?

A young boy’s efforts while learning to walk have suggested a new explanation, in a new journal paper jointly authored by his father and grandfather, both academics at the University of Sydney.

In the latest issue of the scientific journal, Frontiers in Neuroscience, the son-and-father team Mac and Rick Shine suggest that the big difference between humans and other species may lie in how we use our brains for routine tasks.

They advance the idea that the key to exploiting the awesome processing power of our brain’s most distinctive feature – the cortex – may have been to liberate it from the drudgery of controlling routine activities.

And that’s where young Tyler Shine, now two years old, comes into the story. When Tyler was first learning to walk, his doting father and grandfather noticed that every step took Tyler’s full attention.

The image has a brain as the background and show the decent of man diagram in the foreground.
The researchers believe the first pre-humans who began to stand upright faced a new evolutionary pressure not just on their bodies, but on their brains as well. This image is for illustrative purposes only. Credit NeuroscienceNews.com.

But before too long, walking became routine, and Tyler was able to start noticing other things around him. He was better at maintaining his balance, which freed up his attention to focus on more interesting tasks, like trying to get into mischief.

How did Tyler improve? His father and grandfather suggest that he did so by transferring the control of his balance to ‘lower’ parts of the brain, freeing up the powerful cortex to focus on unpredictable challenges, such as a bumpy floor covered in stray toys.

“Any complicated task – like driving a car or playing a musical instrument – starts out consuming all our attention, but eventually becomes routine,” Mac Shine says.

“Studies of brain function suggest that we shift the control of these routine tasks down to ‘lower’ areas of the brain, such as the basal ganglia and the cerebellum.

“So, humans are smart because we have automated the routine tasks; and thus, can devote our most potent mental faculties to deal with new, unpredictable challenges.

“What event in the early history of humans made us change the way we use our brains?

Watching Tyler learn to walk suggested that it was the evolutionary shift from walking on all fours, to walking on two legs.

“Suddenly our brains were overwhelmed with the complicated challenge of keeping our balance – and the best kind of brain to have, was one that didn’t waste its most powerful functions on controlling routine tasks.”

So, the Shines believe, those first pre-humans who began to stand upright faced a new evolutionary pressure not just on their bodies, but on their brains as well.

“New technologies are allowing us to look inside the brain while it works, and we are learning an enormous amount,” Mac Shine says.

“But in order to interpret those results, we need new ideas as well. I’m delighted that my son has played a role in suggesting one of those ideas.

“Hopefully, by the time he is watching his own son learn to walk, we will be much closer to truly understanding the greatest mystery of human existence: how our brains work.”

Notes about this neurobiology research

Contact: Rachel Gleeson – University of Sydney
Source: University of Sydney press release
Image Source: The image is adpated from open source image by Nemo and OpenClips, and was created by NeuroscienceNews.com. We release this image into the public domain.
Original Research: Full open access research for “Delegation to automaticity: the driving force for cognitive evolution?” by J. M. Shine and R. Shine in Frontiers in Neuroscience. Published online April 29 2014 doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00090

Open Access Neuroscience Abstract

Delegation to automaticity: the driving force for cognitive evolution?

The ability to delegate control over repetitive tasks from higher to lower neural centers may be a fundamental innovation in human cognition. Plausibly, the massive neurocomputational challenges associated with the mastery of balance during the evolution of bipedality in proto-humans provided a strong selective advantage to individuals with brains capable of efficiently transferring tasks in this way. Thus, the shift from quadrupedal to bipedal locomotion may have driven the rapid evolution of distinctive features of human neuronal functioning. We review recent studies of functional neuroanatomy that bear upon this hypothesis, and identify ways to test our ideas.

“Delegation to automaticity: the driving force for cognitive evolution?” by J. M. Shine and R. Shine in Frontiers in Neuroscience, April 29 2014 doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00090

Share this Neuroscience News
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.