Quantifying how the brain smells

Summary: The olfactory experience is subjective. The perception of odors depends on the context and prior experience of an individual.

Source: CSHL

Scientists haven’t quite decoded how animals smell, but researchers at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory (CSHL) found that it’s different from previously thought.

To understand how the brain processes and interprets smells, CSHL neuroscientists Florin Albeanu, Alexei Koulakov and colleagues Honggoo Chae, Daniel Kepple, Walter Bast from CSHL, and Venkatesh Murthy from Harvard University, are putting past odor classifying models to the test, and they’re discovering discrepancies.

Their results, published in the journal Nature Neuroscience, differ from other published studies that found predictable relations between molecular properties of odors and activity in the early stages of the olfactory system. The new research found that while there were some correlations between some molecular properties of odors and corresponding neuron activity response, they “held little predictive power when new odor pairs or shuffled properties were tested.”

When it comes to smell, “we don’t really know what the brain is looking for, and we don’t know what physical or chemical features if any, the brain extracts,” Albeanu said.

Generally, scientists know that odor particles first enter through the nasal cavity, where odorant receptors expressed by olfactory receptor neurons in the sensory tissue bind to them. The olfactory bulb, a structure located in the forebrain of mammals, then processes information sent up from the receptors. Afterward, the bulb sends out this information to several higher processing brain areas, including the cerebral cortex. There, the olfactory output messages are further analyzed and broadcast across the brain before they’re conveyed back to the bulb in a feedback loop.

“Rich feedback makes the olfactory system somewhat different from the visual system,” Koulakov said. “Olfactory experience is very subjective, perception of smells actually depends on the context, and on an individual’s prior experience.”

This shows olfactory neurons
Average resting fluorescence multiphoton image in the mitral cell layer (left). Numbers indicate the relative positions of 112 mitral cell bodies in the imaged field of view (right). The image is credited to Albeanu lab/CSHL, 2019.

Factoring these in, Albeanu and Koulakov said it’s probable that the entry-level of olfactory inputs and the further processed bulb outputs care about different aspects of smell.

The “rather unexpected” results of the new research, Koulakov said, are an exciting opportunity to build a more comprehensive and testable computational model for the odor space that captures the differences in informational relevance for scent features across the various levels of olfactory processing.

This paper is the first discovery to come out since the pair received the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Director’s Transformative Research Award for an innovative neuroscience research project on the olfactory system in 2018. Albeanu views these findings as “an opening act” for continued research in this area.

About this neuroscience research article

Source:
CSHL
Media Contacts:
Sara Roncero-Menendez – CSHL
Image Source:
The image is credited to Albeanu lab/CSHL, 2019.

Original Research: Closed access
“Mosaic representations of odors in the input and output layers of the mouse olfactory bulb”. Honggoo Chae, Daniel R. Kepple, Walter G. Bast, Venkatesh N. Murthy, Alexei A. Koulakov & Dinu F. Albeanu.
Nature Neuroscience. doi:10.1038/s41593-019-0442-z

Abstract

Mosaic representations of odors in the input and output layers of the mouse olfactory bulb

The elementary stimulus features encoded by the olfactory system remain poorly understood. We examined the relationship between 1,666 physical–chemical descriptors of odors and the activity of olfactory bulb inputs and outputs in awake mice. Glomerular and mitral and tufted cell responses were sparse and locally heterogeneous, with only a weak dependence of their positions on physical–chemical properties. Odor features represented by ensembles of mitral and tufted cells were overlapping but distinct from those represented in glomeruli, which is consistent with an extensive interplay between feedforward and feedback inputs to the bulb. This reformatting was well described as a rotation in odor space. The physical–chemical descriptors accounted for a small fraction in response variance, and the similarity of odors in the physical–chemical space was a poor predictor of similarity in neuronal representations. Our results suggest that commonly used physical–chemical properties are not systematically represented in bulbar activity and encourage further searches for better descriptors of odor space.

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.