Brain Processes Sentence Structures as Fast as Visual Scenes

Summary: The human brain can detect the structure of a short sentence in as little as 150 milliseconds, the speed of a blink. Using brain imaging, scientists found that the brain’s language comprehension system processes sentences flashed on a screen similarly to how it perceives a visual scene.

Even when sentences have grammatical errors, the brain rapidly detects the structure and sometimes even corrects small mistakes automatically. These findings highlight the brain’s incredible speed in processing language, far beyond what we experience in everyday speech.

Key Facts:

  • The brain can detect sentence structures in just 150 milliseconds.
  • Sentence comprehension at a glance resembles rapid visual scene perception.
  • The brain can automatically correct small grammatical mistakes as it processes.

Source: NYU

Today’s digital media deliver rapid messages—such as phone notifications and text overlays on videos—to our brains at an impressive speed, far faster than spoken words reach us. But can we process these texts as quickly as we can determine the makeup of the visuals that are also a part of our screen life? 

The answer appears to be “yes,” according to new studies by a team of New York University linguistics and psychology researchers. It has discovered that when a brief sentence is flashed, our brains detect its basic linguistic structure extremely quickly—in roughly 150 milliseconds, or about the speed of a blink of an eye.

This shows a brain.
The scientists add that even when a sentence contains an agreement error, with the wrong number marking on the verb (nurses cleans wounds), or lacks a plausible meaning, this rapid structure detection occurs in the left temporal cortex. Credit: Neuroscience News

“Our experiments reveal that the brain’s language comprehension system may be able to perceive language similarly to visual scenes, whose essence can be grasped quickly from a single glance,” says Liina Pylkkänen, a professor in NYU’s Department of Linguistics and Department of Psychology, who led the research, which is reported across papers appearing in Science Advances and the Journal of Neuroscience.

“This means the human brain’s processing capacity for language may be much faster than what we might think—in the amount of time it takes to hear one syllable, the brain can actually detect the structure of a short sentence.”  

The rise of email, followed by social media and smartphones, has shifted our reading experience from a contemplative, leisurely activity to quick and fragmented consumption of digital content, with short messages constantly flashing at us through phone notifications, online platforms, and, perhaps soon, augmented realities. 

“This shift has made it clear that our brains not only have the ability to instinctively process rapid messages, but can also make snap decisions based on them—like whether to keep or delete an email or how to respond to a brief social media update,” explains Pylkkänen.

“But how well do we really understand these quick messages and how do our brains manage them? The fact that our brains can, at least in some way, grasp the meaning of these fast messages from just a single glance may reveal something fundamental about the processing potential of the language system.”

The scientists began their research by considering current scientific explanations of how we understand language, which center on word-by-word sentence processing models. The researchers concluded these don’t effectively account for how quickly our brains can process entire sentences seen at a glance, as opposed to word by word like in speech. 

In seeking a better understanding, the authors conducted a series of experiments, measuring brain activity using magnetoencephalography while participants read word lists that were either grammatical sentences (e.g., nurses clean wounds) or just lists of nouns (e.g., hearts lungs livers).

The results showed that the brain’s left temporal cortex—used for language comprehension—starts distinguishing simple three-word sentences from unstructured word lists as quickly as 130 milliseconds after seeing them. 

“This speed suggests that at-a-glance sentence comprehension may resemble the rapid perception of a visual scene rather than the slower, step-by-step process we associate with spoken language,” explains Pylkkänen.

“In the amount of time that it takes one to hear one syllable, the brain can actually detect the structure of a three-word sentence.”

The scientists add that even when a sentence contains an agreement error, with the wrong number marking on the verb (nurses cleans wounds), or lacks a plausible meaning, this rapid structure detection occurs in the left temporal cortex. 

“This suggests that the signals reflect the detection of basic phrase structure, but not necessarily other aspects of the grammar or meaning,” explains Jacqueline Fallon, the Science Advances study’s first author, who was an NYU researcher at the time of the work and is now a doctoral student at the University of Colorado. 

Related research on these rapid signals in the Journal of Neuroscience, led by NYU graduate student Nigel Flower, further supported this idea. It showed that even small errors in phrase structure—like swapping two adjacent words, “all are cats nice”—cause a drop in the brain’s rapid response.

Such small mistakes can easily go unnoticed by readers. In fact, Flower observed that starting around 400 milliseconds, the brain appears to “correct” the mistake, processing the sentence as if it were fully grammatical. 

“This suggests that the brain not only quickly recognizes phrase structure but also automatically corrects small mistakes,” Flower explains. “This explains why readers often miss minor errors—their brains have already corrected them internally.” 

By flashing the study’s participants entire sentences all at once, as opposed to word by word, the researchers could uncover the brain’s ability to quickly identify basic phrase structure, even if the meaning of the sentence was nonsensical or there was a grammatical mistake that still maintained the right phrase structure.

“These findings may provide valuable insights into the brain’s intrinsic language processing abilities, independent of the usual sequential flow of spoken language,” says Pylkkänen.

Funding: The studies were supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS- 2335767) and the NYU Abu Dhabi Institute.

About this language processing and neuroscience research news

Author: James Devitt
Source: NYU
Contact: James Devitt – NYU
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access.
Language at a glance: How our brains grasp linguistic structure from parallel visual input” by Liina Pylkkänen et al. Science Advances


Abstract

Language at a glance: How our brains grasp linguistic structure from parallel visual input

Human brains grasp the gists of visual scenes from a single glance, but to what extent is this possible for language?

While we typically think of language in terms of sequential speech, our everyday experience involves numerous rapidly flashing written notifications, which we understand instantly.

What do our brains detect in the first few hundred milliseconds after seeing such a stimulus?

We flashed short sentences during magnetoencephalography measurement, revealing sentence-sensitive neural activity in left temporal cortex within 130 milliseconds.

These signals emerged for subject-verb-object sentences regardless of grammatical or semantic well-formedness, suggesting that at-a-glance language comprehension begins by detecting basic phrase structure, independent of meaning or other grammatical details.

Our findings unveil one aspect of how our brains process information rapidly in today’s visually saturated world.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.