Brain Mechanisms Involved in Learning Also Drive Social Conformity

Summary: A brain network implicated in learning by trial and error also becomes engaged when people adapt to social norms, a new study reports. Findings suggest people learn from social information similarly to learning by trial and error.

Source: University of Illinois

Some of the same brain systems known to play a role in learning from trial and error also are engaged when people conform to social norms, scientists report in a new study.

The findings are important, the researchers said, because changing one’s behavior to align with one’s peers can contribute to community-building or – depending on the goals and values of the group – societal breakdown.

The study is reported in the journal Psychophysiology.

“Our results show that other people’s behaviors have profound impacts on our brains, which will use these examples as evidence about what is good or bad,” said University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign graduate student Paul Bogdan, who led the research with U. of I. psychology professors Florin Dolcos and Sanda Dolcos at the Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology. “We see that our drive to conform is a pivotal piece of our psychology, with close links to the important brain systems that support learning.”

The researchers set out to better understand how the brain navigates social situations. To do so, they had participants play a modified version of the “ultimatum game,” in which one player proposes how to split $10 with another. The other player, a responder, can either accept or reject the proposer’s offer. If a responder rejects the proposed split, neither party gets any money.

“The goal of the game is to maximize one’s own profits, so accepting any offer would be the logical way to proceed,” Florin Dolcos said. “But people don’t generally behave that way. If they feel an offer is unfair, they’re more likely to reject it, to punish unfair partners even though this comes at their own expense.”

The participants alternated between serving as proposers and responders. Throughout the experiment, participants interacted with a computer but thought they were dealing with other people.

“This design simulates repeated interactions within a community and sheds light on how cooperative relationships are formed and maintained,” Florin Dolcos said.

The researchers wanted to find the links between how people reacted when their partners accepted or rejected their offers and how their behavior changed after observing the offers as responders.

“Our behavior is … shaped indirectly by observing the actions of our peers, which oftentimes trigger drives to conform and imitate their behavior,” the authors wrote. “A key question is how the neurocognitive systems associated with processing other people’s actions overlap with the machinery dedicated to directly learning from rewards and losses.”

The team used electroencephalograms to track electrical activity in participants’ brains as they played the game. EEGs capture brain responses within hundreds of milliseconds after a stimulus. They also point to brain regions involved in those responses.

“We evaluated how participants responded to acceptances or rejections of their offers, then looked for patterns of brain activity that coincided with their decisions to mimic their partners’ decisions,” Sanda Dolcos said.

This shows a brain
The team saw that participants were influenced by how their negotiation partners behaved. Image is in the public domain

The team saw that participants were influenced by how their negotiation partners behaved.

“They became more generous after receiving generous offers and more selfish after receiving selfish offers,” Bogdan said. “And we found that the drive to conform was linked to the same EEG patterns as changing one’s behavior following acceptances or rejections.

“These results demonstrate that the same brain system guides both pathways to behavior change,” Bogdan said. “This indicates that people learn from social information similarly to how they learn on their own from trial and error.”

The study suggests that understanding how someone sees the world “requires considering the people they associate with,” Bogdan said. “More often than not, these will be their guides to right versus wrong.”

About this social neuroscience research news

Author: Diana Yates
Source: University of Illinois
Contact: Diana Yates – University of Illinois
Image: The image is in the public domain

Original Research: Closed access.
Direct feedback and social conformity promote behavioral change via mechanisms indexed by centroparietal positivity: Electrophysiological evidence from a role-swapping ultimatum game” by Paul Bogdan et al. Psychophysiology


Abstract

Direct feedback and social conformity promote behavioral change via mechanisms indexed by centroparietal positivity: Electrophysiological evidence from a role-swapping ultimatum game

Our behavior is shaped by multiple factors, including direct feedback (seeing the outcomes of our past actions) and social observation (in part, via a drive to conform to other peoples’ behaviors). However, it remains unclear how these two processes are linked in the context of behavioral change.

This is important to investigate, as behavioral change is associated with distinct neural correlates that reflect specific aspects of processing, such as information integration and rule updating.

To clarify whether these processes characterize both direct learning and conformity, we elicited the two within the same task, using a role-swapping version of the Ultimatum Game—a fairness paradigm where subjects decide how to share a pot of money with other players—while electroencephalography (EEG) data were recorded. Behavioral results showed that subjects decided how to divide the pot based on both direct feedback (seeing whether their past proposals were accepted or rejected) and social observation (copying the splits that others just proposed). Converging EEG evidence revealed that increased centroparietal positivity (P2, P3b, and late positivity) indexed behavioral changes motivated by direct feedback and those motivated by drives to conform.

However, exploratory analyses also suggest that these two motivating factors may also be dissociable, and that frontal midline theta oscillations may predict behavioral changes linked to direct feedback but not conformity. Overall, this study provides novel electrophysiological evidence regarding the different forms of behavioral change.

These findings are also relevant for understanding the mechanisms of social information processing that underlie successful cooperation.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.