Babies mimic songs

Summary: Children as young as 15 months can repeat simple melodies they hear hours after exposure to the tune.

Source: Ohio State University

Researchers — and parents — have long known that babies learn to speak by mimicking the words they hear. But a new study shows that babies also might try to imitate the singing they hear in songs.

As part of the study, scientists captured audio of a 15-month-old boy making sounds similar to the beginning of the song “Happy Birthday,” hours after he heard the song played on a toy. An analysis of the sounds showed the boy hitting the first six notes of “Happy Birthday” almost spot-on, in G major.

“We know that throughout the first year of life babies become sophisticated music listeners — they learn a lot about the patterns of pitches and rhythms in music,” said Lucia Benetti, a doctoral student at The Ohio State University School of Music and lead author of the study, which was recently published in the Journal of Research in Music Education. “And infants become better at doing this spontaneously. But we don’t know much about how exactly this happens.”

The study is among the first to measure an infant’s attempt to recreate music by following him for an entire day.

“And what we learned is that in this one case at least, the baby is trying to sing along to songs he’s hearing,” Benetti said.

For the study, Benetti recorded one infant, a 15-month-old boy named James, through one 16-hour period. James wore a small, light recording device throughout the day, which captured every sound he heard and made. Benetti and her adviser, Eugenia Costa-Giomi, a professor of music education at Ohio State, then analyzed that audio data using software designed to measure language — things like the number of adult words the baby heard and tried to say. Benetti also listened to the recording and transcribed the music he heard and the music he made, searching for patterns or places where the child seemed to mimic what he heard happening around him.

That technology has primarily been used in previous research studies to collect data that shows how babies develop language, not to understand how they might begin to learn about music.

James’ parents also logged his primary activities that day — things like napping and meals.

In the morning, James spent about 10 minutes with a toy that played the melody of “Happy Birthday.” Later that evening, the device recorded James making about 10 sounds, lasting about four seconds, that resembled the beginning of “Happy Birthday.”

The researchers shared the recordings with people who didn’t know James and didn’t know what the researchers were studying. Those listeners reported that James was trying to sing “Happy Birthday.”

Shortly after James ate lunch, his mom sang the song “Rain Rain” to him twice. (You know how it goes — “Rain, rain, go away, come again some other day.”) Six hours later, James was playing with his father and started singing a version of “Rain Rain.” (The research notes that he sang it for seven seconds in the key of A-flat major.)

James’ dad immediately picked up on what his son was trying to do, and sang the song back to him. James repeated it.

This shows a toddler in headphones
New study finds that babies repeat melodies and sounds from songs. Image is credited to Alireza Attari.

(James’ dad also made up funny lyrics — “Scrambled eggs, scrambled eggs, makes big muscles in my legs.” James didn’t repeat those.)

The study shows that it’s possible for babies to learn melodies from the music they hear around them, Benetti said. She said future work could examine a larger group of babies, with more data, to see whether James’ response was typical.

“We could try to do it systematically to really start to understand how this learning occurs,” she said. “From this study, we at least know that it happens.”

The takeaway for parents? It can’t hurt to sing to your kids.

“The social aspect of music is important — if a baby sees their mother singing, they know she’s engaging with that song, that she’s enjoying it, and they know it must be important,” Benetti said.

“I think that social context is important. It’s engaging and it’s socially relevant, and for them, that’s enough.”

About this neuroscience research article

Source:
Ohio State University
Media Contacts:
Lucia Benetti – Ohio State University
Image Source:
The image is credited to Alireza Attari.

Original Research: Closed access
“Infant Vocal Imitation of Music”. Lucia Benetti, Eugenia Costa-Giomi.
Journal of Research in Music Education doi:10.1177/0022429419890328.

Abstract

Infant Vocal Imitation of Music

Infant vocal production has been studied mainly from the perspective of language development. We studied it from the perspective of singing development by analyzing a 15-month-old’s imitations of songs. The infant wore a recording device that yielded a continuous, 16-hr audio recording of all the sounds produced by him and around him throughout the day. We listened to the audio file and identified instances in which his unprompted vocalizations resembled songs he had heard earlier. One imitation was recognized by his father, who then sang the song himself and engaged in imitative turn taking with the infant; the other imitation went unnoticed by his parents. Perceptual and acoustic analyses of the imitations and the song models showed that the infant imitated critical music features of the songs, including pitches, intervals, and rhythms. We discuss the use of new technologies for the study of singing development in infancy; such technologies facilitate the collection of spontaneous vocalizations that may go unnoticed by parents and make it possible to trace connections between music environment opportunities and specific singing outcomes in infants.

Feel free to share this Neuroscience and Music News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.