The Social Roots of Treating Animals Like People

Summary: Anthropomorphism—seeing animals as human-like—has long influenced which species receive conservation attention. A new study reveals that social, cultural, and religious factors all shape people’s tendency to attribute human qualities to animals.

Individuals who are socially isolated or raised in urban environments were more likely to see animals as conscious or emotional beings. However, formal education and monotheistic religions were linked to lower tendencies to assign animals free will or complex emotions.

Key Facts:

  • Social Isolation Effect: Lonely individuals are more likely to anthropomorphize animals.
  • Urban Influence: Pet ownership, zoos, and media exposure boost human-like views of animals.
  • Religious Impact: Monotheistic beliefs reduce attributions of free will and consciousness in animals.

Source: Cell Press

From depressed polar bears to charismatic pandas, conservationists have used anthropomorphism, or the practice of attributing human qualities to non-human subjects, to garner public support for conservation efforts.

In a new study publishing June 18 in the Cell Press journal iScience, scientists tease apart some of the social factors that influence whether people view animals similarly to humans.

This shows cute animals.
Taken together, these results help identify communities where attributing human traits to animals may enhance conservation strategies. Credit: Neuroscience News.

The researchers found that factors such as social integration, urban living, formal education, and religion can affect an individual’s tendency to assign human characteristics to animals.

This in turn may affect their willingness to engage with conservation programs.  

“Unfortunately, anthropomorphism significantly influences conservation efforts,” says author Federica Amici of Leipzig University.

“Species that appear more human-like or exhibit human-like behaviors usually tend to receive more attention, funding, and public support. This preference can overshadow the ecological importance of less charismatic species, undermining broader conservation goals.”

To understand the personal factors that may influence an individual’s tendency to anthropomorphize, the research team conducted a survey of 741 adults from Brazil, Indonesia, Mexico, Malaysia, and Spain.

The questionnaire focused on participants’ upbringing as well as on their perceptions about animals’ physical similarity to humans and their beliefs about animals’ capacity for emotion, consciousness, and free will.

The survey found that socially isolated people more often associate animals with human emotions and physical similarity than their more socially connected counterparts.

“People who feel lonely or aren’t well connected to others often try to meet their need for social connection by seeing human-like qualities in animals or other non-human things,” says Amici.

“For example, there is literature showing that individuals who are chronically lonely are more likely to treat their pets as if they have human thoughts and feelings than those who have strong social ties.”

Those with more “urban” experiences—such as having a pet, visiting zoos, or consuming media with animals—had the strongest trend toward anthropomorphism across the survey.

These participants were more likely to assign consciousness, emotions, and intentionality to animals than those with fewer of these interactions. In contrast, those with greater formal education were less likely to think animals had autonomy.

Religious upbringing also had an impact, with participants of monotheistic faiths like Islam and Christianity being less likely to ascribe consciousness or free will to animals in comparison to participants from religions such has Buddhism and Hinduism. Agnostics and atheists also gave less free will to animals on average, with a score comparable to those of Christian faith.

“While we know that religious systems provide comprehensive sets of values, norms, and beliefs that shape how people relate to nature and animals, I was struck by the clear difference between monotheistic and non-monotheistic religions in terms of anthropomorphic tendencies,” says Amici.

“Non-monotheistic religions appear to contribute more strongly to envisioning a space of coexistence between humans and other species.”

Taken together, these results help identify communities where attributing human traits to animals may enhance conservation strategies. However, Amici notes that identifying the underpinnings of anthropomorphism should not be the primary aim.

“I think anthropomorphizing species can be an effective approach, but it should be used with caution,” says Amici.

“This could be counterbalanced by emphasizing other aspects, such as the species’ crucial ecological role. Whatever approach conservationists take, I believe it is essential to remain aware of the many cognitive and emotional biases that shape human perceptions of nature and other animals.”

The researchers noted that while the study captures larger trends, the results are not representative of the full breadth of people’s views and experiences surrounding anthropomorphism.

About this anthropomorphism and psychology research news

Author: Jordan Greer
Source: Cell Press
Contact: Jordan Greer – Cell Press
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access.
Experience with animals, religion and social integration predict anthropomorphism across five countries” by Federica Amici et al. iScience


Abstract

Experience with animals, religion and social integration predict anthropomorphism across five countries

Conservation efforts are largely dependent on the amount of public concern for wildlife protection. One of the factors that best predict willingness to support conservation projects is anthropomorphism.

Here, we administered questionnaires to a cross-cultural sample (N = 741), including adult participants from Brazil, Indonesia, Malaysia, Mexico, and Spain, to investigate the drivers of inter-individual variation in anthropomorphism.

Anthropomorphism increased when participants had more “urban” experience with animals, lower social integration, higher allocentric tendencies, and lower formal education.

Participants with higher exposure to monkeys were also less likely to attribute them the ability to feel pain, while considering them accountable for their actions.

Religions emphasizing human similarity and inter-connection of all living beings predicted higher anthropomorphism, with the specific taxa and traits considered modulating these effects.

Overall, this work contributes to better understanding the factors that explain variation in anthropomorphism and that might promote interest in other species and foster conservation efforts.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.