Summary: A new study presents the first quantitative comparison of adaptation response.
Source: University of Toronto
Research on sensory adaptation led by University of Toronto Engineering professor Willy Wong may have unearthed a previously overlooked organizational principle of physiology.
Biologists have long known that organisms adapt to a constant stimulus in a similar way, says Wong.
“Imagine you walk into a room someone has just painted. You’ll likely think, ‘This smells bad.’ But the sensation decreases as you stay in there. The molecules don’t disappear, not within that time frame. You’ve just gotten used to it.”
From an initial state, the organism’s response activity rises to a peak response, then falls to a new final steady state. Wong has discovered that those three fixed points on the adaptation curve form a mathematical relationship that is obeyed across all sensory modalities and organisms.
“I compared 250 measurements of adaptation from different branches of sensory physiology and found that they are all compatible with a single, simple equation,” says Wong.
His findings, the first quantitative comparison of adaptation responses, are presented in a paper in Frontiers of Human Neuroscience.
Wong’s recent work in brain-machine interfaces, such as a retinal prosthesis to restore vision for blind patients, builds on his long-standing fascination with the neural code—how neurons process information. Though today’s understanding of the code remains far from perfect, the more researchers understand how our brains convert signals into perceptions, the better they can design technologies to replace lost functions or enhance existing ones.
The idea of a sensory response curve that drops off over time might seem counterintuitive: Shouldn’t a strong sensation return a consistently strong rate of response? But as long ago as the 1920s, physiologists such as Edgar Adrian were demonstrating why not.
Adrian, whose work would win the 1932 Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine, used a frog specimen to trace the adaptation phenomenon to the level of single neurons. He discovered that neurons use a basic unit of communication, a nerve impulse called an action potential, which fires the same signal strength as long as a threshold is reached.
“Action potentials don’t come in half measures,” says Wong. “Either you get one or you don’t. If you do, the neuron needs some time to recharge before it can fire another. In adaptation, the rate of action potential generation falls gradually to some non-zero steady state.”
Adaptation response occurs in all animals, from vertebrates like mammals to invertebrates like insects, and across all sensory modalities. This includes the five traditional senses of vision, hearing, touch, taste and smell, along with somatosensory functions such as proprioception—the body’s awareness of itself—and electroreception, as found in eels.
One of Wong’s biggest surprises was that his equation holds true for some of the oldest multicellular organisms, such as jellyfish, which have very different sensory systems.
“If you shine a light on them, they either fly to the light or away from it—but only because their photoreceptors are hardwired to their motor output,” he says. “Which raises the question, is this equation universal? In the future, if we find aliens with exobiology never seen on this planet, could they also be constrained by the same limitations or principles?”
In the physical sciences, universality is determined by replication of results, regardless of when, where, or by what method they are obtained. But this is not always possible in biological experiments, which can pose significant barriers to repetition of measurements.
However, when data from unrelated independent studies—across different time periods, researchers and methods—converge as evidence, it strengthens the case for the conclusion. This principle, called consilience, is based on the premise that science is unified, bolstering consensus in theories such as evolutionary theory and the big bang theory, among others.
“All this data was there,” says Wong, “I took a curve here, a curve there, compared them—even Adrian’s canonical graphs. All conformed to the same geometric mean relationship. It’s not dependent on the researcher, on what equipment was used, or on the organism. From that perspective, it is universal.”
“This is illuminating work from Professor Wong,” says Professor Deepa Kundur, Chair of Electrical & Computer Engineering at the University of Toronto. “It’s a reminder of just how pervasive electrical and computer engineering is—how researchers are able to contribute to many seemingly far-reaching areas of study.”
The discovery of a new physiological equation doesn’t happen every day, and it’s unlikelier still to come from an engineer. Though Wong had been developing these ideas for years, he credits the pandemic with giving him some time to refocus, as well as fruitful periods of research progress.
“I was on the elliptical,” he says, when asked to pinpoint his “a-ha” moment. “Either reading news or thinking about my work. I think that was the moment.”
About this neuroscience research news
Author: Matthew Tierney
Source: University of Toronto
Contact: Matthew Tierney – University of Toronto
Image: The image is credited to Willy Wong / University of Toronto Engineering
Original Research: Open access.
“Consilience in the Peripheral Sensory Adaptation Response” by Willy Wong. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
Abstract
Consilience in the Peripheral Sensory Adaptation Response
Measurements of the peripheral sensory adaptation response were compared to a simple mathematical relationship involving the spontaneous, peak, and steady-state activities.
This relationship is based on the geometric mean and is found to be obeyed to good approximation in peripheral sensory units showing a sustained response to prolonged stimulation.
From an extensive review of past studies, the geometric mean relationship is shown to be independent of modality and is satisfied in a wide range of animal species.
The consilience of evidence, from nearly 100 years of experiments beginning with the work of Edgar Adrian, suggests that this is a fundamental result of neurophysiology.