For Twins, Gesture and Speech Go Hand-in-Hand In Language Development

Summary: Twins lag behind single-born children when it comes to language development and hand gestures. However, female twins develop communication skills slightly faster than male twins.

Source: Georgia State University

Gestures—such as pointing or waving—go hand in hand with a child’s first words, and twins lag behind single children in producing and using those gestures, two studies from Georgia State University psychology researchers show.

Twins produce fewer gestures and gesture to fewer objects than other children, said principal researcher Seyda Ozcaliskan, an associate professor in the Department of Psychology. Language use also lags for twins, and language—but not gesture—is also affected by sex, with girls performing better than boys, Ozcaliskan said.

“The implications are fascinating,” said Ozcaliskan. “It shows that gesture and speech go hand in hand in early development in twins. When one is lagging behind so does the other.”

The research found that a lag in gesture can reliably predict a lag in speech. At the same time, lots of gesturing suggests speech is on the way. Parents can help speed their child’s acquisition of language by naming the objects they gesture to.

The lag in gesture among twins may be due to lower parental input, Ozcaliskan said, for parents of single children used a greater amount and diversity of gestures than parents of twins. It’s likely, Ozcaliskan said, that parents caring for twins engage in shorter conversations with their children, including gesturing less, because their attention is divided and their labor is doubled.

The studies were conducted by Ozcaliskan and her psychology doctoral students Ebru Pinar and Sumeyra Ozturk, along with her collaborator Dr. Nihan Ketrez in Istanbul.

The paper Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins was published in the Journal of Nonverbal Behavior and Effect of Sex and Dyad Composition on Speech and Gesture Development of Singleton and Twin Children was published in the Journal of Child Language.

The two studies analyzed video data from Turkish families and included three groups of fraternal twins–males, females and mixed sex twins, as well as two groups of single boys and girls, along with their parents.

Ozcaliskan said she had grown curious about gesture because it had not yet been studied among twins, while language acquisition has been thoroughly researched.

“We’ve known for a long time that children’s early vocabulary shows sex differences,” said Ozcaliskan, “with boys having smaller vocabularies than girls their age.”

Girls age 2-3 also produce longer, more complex sentences than boys. Twins are initially at a disadvantage where language is concerned, using fewer words than their singleton peers and forming complete sentences later than singletons. Boys lag the most, with girl-girl twins developing larger vocabularies and more complex sentences than boy-boy twins of comparable age.

Most of the time, these lags are within the normal range of differences, and nothing to worry about. Almost all children will catch up to each other by around age three and a half.

But what about gesture?

“There was close to nothing in research on gesturing among twins in any language,” Ozcaliskan said.

Infants start using pointing around 10 months, a few months before they produce their first words. In other words, they point at a dog a few months before they actually say the word “dog.” Gesture helps them convey what they can’t yet communicate in speech and paves the way for children’s early words.

Though the lag in gesture and language among twins may be mostly due to reduced parental input, Ozcaliskan said twins sometimes develop their own systems of communication, even their own “twin language,” which has been shown to delay language acquisition. “Perhaps it also delays the use of gestures,” she said.

The research found that a lag in gesture can reliably predict a lag in speech. At the same time, lots of gesturing suggests speech is on the way. Image is in the public domain

On most occasions, a lag in gesture may mean a lag in the next linguistic milestone.

“However, in the event that you see the lag in gesture go on for a long time, it may be a marker of a potential developmental or language delay,” said Ozcaliskan. On the other hand, if a child doesn’t talk much at an early age, but frequently uses gesture, a parent can be reassured that “language is on its way,” she said.

The more a parent engages with a child, using gesture and speech, the better the child’s acquisition of both. Gesture and speech together form a closely integrated system in a child’s development, and a child’s first gestures often precede their first attempts at speech.

For parents who are intrigued by these connections, Ozcaliskan suggested naming objects when children point to them, as in: Yes, that’s a bottle, do you want your bottle? Naming helps children learn new words earlier. Parents can also make a point of gesturing to objects themselves when naming.

“Gesture is a very powerful tool,” Ozcaliskan said. “Pay attention to your child’s gestures, and then provide verbal descriptions to help their language development.”

About this language research news

Source: Georgia State University
Contact: Anna Varela – Georgia State University
Image: The image is in the public domain

Original Research: Closed access.
Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins” by Seyda Ozcaliskan et al. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior


Abstract

Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins

Children show sex differences in early speech development, with girls producing a greater number and variety of words at an earlier age than boys (Berglund et al. in Scand J Psychol 46(6): 485–491, 2005)—a pattern that also becomes evident in gesture (Butterworth and Morisette in J Reprod Infant Psychol 14(3): 219–231, 1996). Importantly, parents show variability in how they produce speech when interacting with their singleton sons vs. daughters (i.e., Cherry and Lewis in Dev Psychol 12: 278–282, 1976; Leaper et al. in Dev Psychol 34: 3–27, 1998).

However, it is unknown whether the variability in speech input extends to different twin dyads or becomes evident in gesture input. In this study, we examined parental gesture and speech input to 35 singleton (19 boys, 16 girls) and 62 twin (10 boy–boy, 9 girl–girl, and 12 girl–boy dyads) Turkish children (age range = 0;10–3;4) in parent–child interactions. We asked whether there is evidence of sex (girls vs. boys) or group (singletons vs. twins) differences in parents’ speech and gesture production, and whether these differences also become evident in different twin dyads (girl–girl, boy–boy, girl–boy).

Our results, based on parent-child interactions, largely showed no evidence of sex or dyad-composition difference in either parent speech or gesture, but evidence of a group difference in gesture, with the parents of singletons providing a greater amount, diversity, and complexity of gestures than parents of twins in their interactions.

These results suggest that differences in parent input to singletons vs. twins might become evident initially in gesture.

Join our Newsletter
Thank you for subscribing.
Something went wrong.
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
Exit mobile version