Refresh

This website neurosciencenews.com/ability-estimation-18897/ is currently offline. Cloudflare's Always Online™ shows a snapshot of this web page from the Internet Archive's Wayback Machine. To check for the live version, click Refresh.

Why Some People Overestimate Their Abilities While Others Underestimate

Summary: Lacking confidence in our own ability may stem from overestimating the abilities and performance of others, researchers say.

Source: University of Alberta

A lack of confidence in our abilities on a given task or activity seems to stem from overestimating the abilities of others, according to a University of Alberta study.

The finding could offer leaders insights into how to counter self-doubt in the face of a difficult task.

Previous research has shown that for many tasks and activities, the majority of people tend to predict that they will outperform others, especially when tasks are easy. A classic example comes from a 1981 study of U.S. drivers in which 93 percent claimed they were better than average.

On difficult tasks, however, most people tend to predict that others will do better than they will.

In making sense of these seemingly contradictory findings, the study surveyed runners before a timed race about how they expected to do.

The researchers—Gerald Häubl, a marketing professor in the Alberta School of Business and the Ronald K. Banister Chair in Business, and Isabelle Engeler of the University of Navarra in Spain—chose a challenging mountain race, with uphill distances ranging from 10 to 78 kilometres.

Controlling for age, gender and running experience, the researchers found that the runners who wrongly predicted that their finishing times would be better than average—those who were overconfident—were driven mainly by overestimating their own performances.

Meanwhile, runners who predicted they would perform worse than average—those who were underconfident in their abilities—had a solid understanding of their own performance but expected more from their competitors.

“Our work identifies two distinct sources of bias or two different reasons for why people might not be well calibrated: they can be biased in their self-assessment, and they can be biased in their assessment of others,” said Häubl.

As well, the underconfident group was not only quite accurate in predicting their own performance, they also tended to be those who were better than average.

Häubl said underconfidence, which can manifest itself in the workplace as imposter syndrome, is often beneficial, particularly if it motivates people to work harder.

This shows a woman about to run a race
In a study of competitive runners, marketing researchers found that those who overestimated their ability actually tended to perform below average, whereas those who lacked confidence tended to overestimate their competitors’ ability—two distinct biases that offer lessons for leaders. Credit: University of Alberta

“The problem with underconfidence, however, is that it can prevent people who actually have the potential to excel at something—a particular job or career—from even trying, because they falsely believe there are many others who are better than they are.”

Likewise, individuals who overestimate their performance tend to be those who are worse than average.

“This latter result bears parallels with prior research showing that people who are unskilled tend to overestimate their performance,” said Häubl.

This overconfidence, he said, can be good or bad, depending on whether it translates into higher or lower motivation and thus a desired outcome.

“Some of humankind’s greatest achievements were probably fuelled by some form of overconfidence. But then, so were some of humankind’s most spectacular failures,” he said.

“In very general terms, well-calibrated confidence, based on an accurate assessment of both one’s own and others’ abilities, is what people should strive for.”

About this psychology research news

Source: University of Alberta
Contact: Michael Brown – University of Alberta
Image: The image is credited to University of Alberta

Original Research: Closed access.
Miscalibration in predicting one’s performance: Disentangling misplacement and misestimation” by Engeler, I., & Häubl, G. Journal of Social Psychology and Personality


Abstract

Miscalibration in predicting one’s performance: Disentangling misplacement and misestimation

When people predict their performance, they can be miscalibrated in two ways—they may mispredict how they will perform relative to others (misplacement) and how they will perform in absolute terms (misestimation). Prior work has yielded contradictory conclusions about the relative direction of these two types of miscalibration.

Some research found that they occur in opposite directions—that is, that people who believe they are better than average (BTA) tend to underestimate their absolute performance, whereas those who believe they are worse than average (WTA) tend to overestimate their performance.

Other studies found that the two types of miscalibration occur in the same direction—that is, that people with BTA beliefs tend to overestimate their performance. We reconcile these apparently conflicting findings by disentangling placement beliefs that are correct from those that are erroneous and focusing on the latter as only these represent instances of misplacement.

Two field studies reveal a key asymmetry—erroneous BTA beliefs are primarily driven by misestimation of one’s own absolute performance, whereas erroneous WTA beliefs tend to be driven by misestimation of others’ absolute performance. A reexamination of data from Moore and Small (2007) supports this insight about the sources of misplacement beliefs.

The findings suggest that the conflicting conclusions from prior work might have been attributable to differences in the extent to which results were based on observations with erroneous (as opposed to correct) placement beliefs.

This research provides novel insights into the psychology of miscalibration in performance predictions and helps unify seemingly contradictory prior findings.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. By exclusively using runners, the study has already biased itself and results are probably useless. Conclusions should be some runners believe others are faster than them, and they are correct; some runners believe others are slower than them, and they are correct; and the permutations of those.

Comments are closed.