Why Some Songs Get Stuck in Our Head

Summary: A new study finds those earworm songs that get stuck in our heads are usually faster, fairly generic and easier to remember, but with unique intervals that set them apart from ‘average’ pop songs.

Source: Durham University.

Almost all of us get songs stuck in our heads from time to time but why do certain tunes have the ‘stick factor’?

The first large-scale study, led by Dr Kelly Jakubowski at Durham University, may have some answers to this musical stickiness.

It has shown that songs that get stuck in your head – called earworms or involuntary musical imagery – are usually faster, with a fairly generic and easy-to-remember melody but with some unique intervals such as leaps or repetitions that set it apart from the “average pop song”.

Prime examples of earworms named in the study include Bad Romance by Lady Gaga, Don’t Stop Believing by Journey and perhaps not surprisingly Can’t Get You Out Of My Head by Kylie Minogue.

It is often assumed that songs that get more radio time and have more recently featured in the charts are more likely to be reported as earworms. The current study has also confirmed this idea by testing it scientifically for the first time.

However, there has previously been limited evidence about what makes the actual song catchy regardless of popularity or how often people may have heard it.

Lead author Dr Kelly Jakubowski from the Department of Music at Durham University, said: “Our findings show that you can to some extent predict which songs are going to get stuck in people’s heads based on the song’s melodic content. This could help aspiring song-writers or advertisers write a jingle everyone will remember for days or months afterwards.

“These musically sticky songs seem to have quite a fast tempo along with a common melodic shape and unusual intervals or repetitions like we can hear in the opening riff of Smoke On The Water by Deep Purple or in the chorus of Bad Romance by Lady Gaga.”

The study, published in the academic journal Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, was conducted by researchers based at Durham University, Goldsmiths, University of London and the University of Tübingen in Germany with funding from the Leverhulme Trust.

The current study found that the tunes most likely to get stuck in people’s heads were those with more common global melodic contours, meaning they have very typical overall melodic shapes commonly found in pop music.

An example of one of the most common contour patterns in Western music is that heard in Twinkle Twinkle Little Star where the first phrase rises in pitch and the second falls. Numerous other nursery rhymes follow the same pattern making it easy for young children to remember. The opening riff of Moves Like Jagger by Maroon 5, one of the top-named earworm tunes in the study, also follows this common contour pattern of rising then falling in pitch.

In addition to a common melodic shape, the other crucial ingredient in the earworm formula is an unusual interval structure in the song such as some unexpected leaps or more repeated notes than you would expect to hear in the “average pop song”. The instrumental riff of My Sharona by the Knack and In The Mood by Glen Miller both have this unusual interval structure.

Dr Jakubowski, who conducted the research whilst based at Goldsmiths, University of London but has since joined the Department of Music at Durham University, added: “We already know that recent and frequent exposure to a song makes it more likely to get stuck in your head and people who sing and listen to music a lot tend to get earworms more often than others.

“We now also know that, regardless of the chart success of a song, there are certain features of the melody that make it more prone to getting stuck in people’s heads like some sort of private musical screensaver.”

The researchers asked 3,000 people for their most frequent earworm tunes and compared these to tunes which had never been named as earworms in the database but were a match in terms of popularity and how recently they had been in the UK Music Charts. The melodic features of the earworm and non-earworm tunes were then analysed and compared. Songs were limited to popular music genres, such as pop, rock, rap, rhythm & blues.

Studies of earworms can help to understand how brain networks, which are involved in perception, emotions, memory and spontaneous thoughts, behave in different people.

Image shows a woman with a headset on.
Prime examples of earworms named in the study include Bad Romance by Lady Gaga, Don’t Stop Believing by Journey and perhaps not surprisingly Can’t Get You Out Of My Head by Kylie Minogue.
NeuroscienceNews.com image is for illustrative purposes only.

Ninety per cent of us get a song stuck in our heads playing on an endless loop at least once a week. It normally happens at times when the brain is not doing much such as in the shower, whilst walking or doing chores.

As well as recent and frequent exposure to songs, words, images or other associations can also dig up a song in our brains out of nowhere and lead it to play in our mind over and over again.

The data for the study was collected from 2010 to 2013.

Most frequently named earworms in study

  • Bad Romance – Lady Gaga
  • Can’t Get You Out Of My Head – Kylie Minogue
  • Don’t Stop Believing – Journey
  • Somebody That I Used To Know – Gotye
  • Moves Like Jagger – Maroon 5
  • California Gurls – Katy Perry
  • Bohemian Rhapsody – Queen
  • Alejandro – Lady Gaga
  • Poker Face – Lady Gaga

How to get rid of an earworm

  • Engage with the song: Many people report that actually listening to the earworm song all the way through can help to eliminate having it stuck on a loop.
  • Distract yourself by thinking of or listening to a different song. The top-named “cure song” for displacing earworms is God Save the Queen.
  • Let it be: Others find that the best way to get rid of an earworm is to just try not to think about it and let it fade away naturally on its own.
  • About this psychology research article

    Funding: Funding provided by Leverhulme Trust.

    Source: Dionne Hamil – Durham University
    Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
    Original Research: Abstract for “Dissecting an Earworm: Melodic Features and Song Popularity Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery” by Jakubowski, Kelly; Finkel, Sebastian; Stewart, Lauren; and Müllensiefen, Daniel in Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts. Published online November 3 2016 doi:10.1037/aca0000090

    Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

    [cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]Durham University. “Why Some Songs Get Stuck in Our Head.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 3 November 2016.
    <https://neurosciencenews.com/song-stuck-in-head-5423/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]Durham University. (2016, November 3). Why Some Songs Get Stuck in Our Head. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved November 3, 2016 from https://neurosciencenews.com/song-stuck-in-head-5423/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]Durham University. “Why Some Songs Get Stuck in Our Head.” https://neurosciencenews.com/song-stuck-in-head-5423/ (accessed November 3, 2016).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


    Abstract

    Dissecting an Earworm: Melodic Features and Song Popularity Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery

    Involuntary musical imagery (INMI or “earworms”)—the spontaneous recall and repeating of a tune in one’s mind—can be attributed to a wide range of triggers, including memory associations and recent musical exposure. The present study examined whether a song’s popularity and melodic features might also help to explain whether it becomes INMI, using a dataset of tunes that were named as INMI by 3,000 survey participants. It was found that songs that had achieved greater success and more recent runs in the U.K. music charts were reported more frequently as INMI. A set of 100 of these frequently named INMI tunes was then matched to 100 tunes never named as INMI by the survey participants, in terms of popularity and song style. These 2 groups of tunes were compared using 83 statistical summary and corpus-based melodic features and automated classification techniques. INMI tunes were found to have more common global melodic contours and less common average gradients between melodic turning points than non-INMI tunes, in relation to a large pop music corpus. INMI tunes also displayed faster average tempi than non-INMI tunes. Results are discussed in relation to literature on INMI, musical memory, and melodic “catchiness.”

    “Dissecting an Earworm: Melodic Features and Song Popularity Predict Involuntary Musical Imagery” by Jakubowski, Kelly; Finkel, Sebastian; Stewart, Lauren; and Müllensiefen, Daniel in Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts. Published online November 3 2016 doi:10.1037/aca0000090

    Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
    Join our Newsletter
    I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
    Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
    We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.