Reading Between the Lines in Children’s Vocabulary Differences

Summary: Children whose families have a lower socioeconomic status have significantly smaller vocabularies than their more affluent peers, a new study reports.

Source: UT Dallas.

The nation’s 31 million children growing up in homes with low socioeconomic status have, on average, significantly smaller vocabularies compared with their peers.

A new study from the Callier Center for Communication Disorders at The University of Texas at Dallas found these differences in vocabulary growth among grade school children of different socioeconomic statuses are likely related to differences in the process of word learning.

Dr. Mandy Maguire, associate professor in the School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences (BBS), said in her study that children from lower-income homes learned 10 percent fewer words than their peers from higher-income homes. When entering kindergarten, children from low-income homes generally score about two years behind their higher-income peers on language and vocabulary measures.

Image shows a book.
The primary reason for the differences in infancy and preschool is related to different quantity and quality of language exposure at home. But why the gap increases as the children get older is less studied. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to UT Dallas.

The vocabulary gap between the two groups of children gets larger throughout their schooling and has long-term academic implications, Maguire said.

The primary reason for the differences in infancy and preschool is related to different quantity and quality of language exposure at home. But why the gap increases as the children get older is less studied.

“We might assume that it’s the same reason that the gap is large when they’re young: that their environment is different,” Maguire said. “Another possibility is that all of this time spent in low-income situations has led to differences in their ability to learn a word. If that’s the case, there’s a problem in the mechanism of learning, which is something we can fix.”

The study, recently published in the Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, aimed to determine whether socioeconomic status is related to word learning in grade school and to what degree vocabulary, reading and working memory might mediate that relationship.

For the study, 68 children ages 8 to 15 performed a task that required using the surrounding text to identify the meaning of an unknown word. One exercise included three sentences, each with a made-up word at the end — for example, “Mom piled the pillows on the thuv.”

“You have to understand all of the language in each sentence leading up to the made-up word, remember it and decide systematically across all three sentences what the made-up word must mean,” Maguire said. “In this case, the three sentences all indicated ‘thuv’ meant ‘bed.’ This isn’t quite the same as real word learning, where we have to create a new concept, but this what we think kids — and adults — do as they initially learn a word.”

Specifically, the study found that children of lower socioeconomic status are not as effective at using known vocabulary to build a robust picture or concept of the incoming language and use that to identify the meaning of an unknown word.

Reading and working memory — also known to be problematic for children from low-income homes — were not found to be related.

The study also provides potential strategies that may be effective for intervention. For children ages 8 to 15, schools may focus too much on reading and not enough on increasing vocabulary through oral methods, Maguire said.

Maguire said parents and teachers can help children identify relationships between words in sentences, such as assigning a word like “bakery,” and having the child list as many related words as possible in one minute. Visualizing the sentences as they read also can help.

“Instead of trying to fit more vocabulary in a child’s head, we might be able to work on their depth of knowledge of the individual words and linking known meanings together in a way that they can use to learn new information,” Maguire said.

About this neuroscience research article

Funding: This study was funded by a three-year grant from the National Science Foundation, which was awarded in April 2016.

Three co-authors of the paper are BBS doctoral students who work in Maguire’s Developmental Neurolinguistics Laboratory: Julie M. Schneider, Anna E. Middleton and Yvonne Ralph. Lab coordinator Michael Lopez and Dr. Robert Ackerman, an associate professor, also are co-authors, along with Dr. Alyson Abel, a recent Callier Center postdoctoral fellow who is an assistant professor at San Diego State University.

Source: Brittany Magelssen – UT Dallas
Publisher: Organized by NeuroscienceNews.com.
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to UT Dallas.
Original Research: Abstract for “Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school” by Mandy J. Maguire, Julie M. Schneider, Anna E. Middleton, Yvonne Ralph, Michael Lopez, Robert A. Ackerman, and Alyson D. Abel in Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Published online November 2 2017 doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2017.10.003

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]UT Dallas “Reading Between the Lines in Children’s Vocabulary Differences.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 30 November 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/childrens-vocabulary-8073/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]UT Dallas (2017, November 30). Reading Between the Lines in Children’s Vocabulary Differences. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved November 30, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/childrens-vocabulary-8073/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]UT Dallas “Reading Between the Lines in Children’s Vocabulary Differences.” https://neurosciencenews.com/childrens-vocabulary-8073/ (accessed November 30, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school

The relationship between children’s slow vocabulary growth and the family’s low socioeconomic status (SES) has been well documented. However, previous studies have often focused on infants or preschoolers and primarily used static measures of vocabulary at multiple time points. To date, there is no research investigating whether SES predicts a child’s word learning abilities in grade school and, if so, what mediates this relationship. In this study, 68 children aged 8–15 years performed a written word learning from context task that required using the surrounding text to identify the meaning of an unknown word. Results revealed that vocabulary knowledge significantly mediated the relationship between SES (as measured by maternal education) and word learning. This was true despite the fact that the words in the linguistic context surrounding the target word are typically acquired well before 8 years of age. When controlling for vocabulary, word learning from written context was not predicted by differences in reading comprehension, decoding, or working memory. These findings reveal that differences in vocabulary growth between grade school children from low and higher SES homes are likely related to differences in the process of word learning more than knowledge of surrounding words or reading skills. Specifically, children from lower SES homes are not as effective at using known vocabulary to build a robust semantic representation of incoming text to identify the meaning of an unknown word.

“Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school” by Mandy J. Maguire, Julie M. Schneider, Anna E. Middleton, Yvonne Ralph, Michael Lopez, Robert A. Ackerman, and Alyson D. Abel in Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Published online November 2 2017 doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2017.10.003

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.