Making Us Blush: Study Explores Blushing Mechanisms

Summary: Researchers investigated the neural basis of blushing using MRI scans and cheek temperature measurements.

The study found that blushing activates the cerebellum and early visual areas, but not regions linked to understanding mental states. This suggests blushing may be an automatic emotional response rather than a cognitive one.

The findings could help address social anxiety related to blushing.

Key Facts:

  1. Blushing activates the cerebellum and early visual areas.
  2. Cognitive skills may not be necessary for blushing.
  3. Understanding blushing mechanisms can help treat social anxiety.

Source: KNAW

A new collaboration between researchers from the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, the University of Amsterdam and the University of Chieti explores the neural substrates of blushing in a MRI scanner.

Most of us know what it feels like to blush. The face becomes warm and red, and we experience self-conscious emotions, such as embarrassment, shyness, shame, and pride.

It is perhaps no wonder that Charles Darwin referred to it as “the most peculiar and the most human of all expressions”. But why do we blush, and what are the underlying mechanisms of blushing?

This shows a woman blushing.
Blushing was associated with increased activity in the cerebellum, an area most known for its role in movement and coordination. Credit: Neuroscience News

To answer this question, Milica Nikolic and Disa Sauter form the University of Amsterdam collaborated with Simone di Plinio from the University of Chieti under the supervision of Christian Keysers and Valeria Gazzola from the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience.

“Blushing is a really interesting phenomenon because we still don’t know which cognitive skills are needed for it to occur”, developmental psychologist Nikolic explains.

“There’s this idea in psychology that dates back to Darwin, who said that blushing occurs when we think about what other people think of us, which involves relatively complex cognitive skills”.

Blushing in a karaoke setting

The researchers investigated blushing by looking at the activated brain areas in an MRI scanner while measuring the cheek temperature—an indicator of blushing. Their participants were female adolescents, a group known to be particularly sensitive to social judgement.

“It is known that blushing increases during this life stage, since adolescents are very sensitive to other people’s opinions and can be afraid of rejection or leaving wrong impression” Nikolic explains.

To evoke a blushing response in a controlled experimental setting, participants came to the lab for two separate sessions. During the first session, they were asked to sing purposefully-chosen difficult karaoke songs and, in the second session, they watched recordings of their own singing while their brain activity and physiological responses were measured.

Adding salt to the wound, they were also told that an audience would watch their recording with them. Finally, the participants were shown recordings from another participant who had sung at comparable level, and a professional singer who was disguised as a third participant.

The mechanism behind blushing

As expected, the researchers found that participants blushed more while watching their own recordings in comparison to other participants’  recordings.

The correlating brain areas were somewhat surprising. Blushing was associated with increased activity in the cerebellum, an area most known for its role in movement and coordination.

“Lately, there has been a lot of research suggesting its involvement in emotional processing”, Nikolic adds.

The researchers also found increased activation in the early visual areas, suggesting that the videos of own singing captured blushing participants’ attention the most.

Surprisingly, there was no activation in areas that are traditionally known to be involved in the process of understanding the mental state of oneself or others.

“Based on this we concluded that thinking about others’ thoughts may not be necessary for blushing to occur” Nikolic concludes. “Blushing may be a part of the automatic arousal you feel when you are exposed and there is something that is relevant to the self”.

Universal phenomenon

Nikolic: “The next step would be to look at blushing under different conditions, or perhaps, even explore the phenomenon in younger children, before they have developed the cognitive skills to think about other people’s thoughts.”

“Blushing in itself is very interesting because it’s universal. There are even people who develop a phobia of blushing, for example, people with a social anxiety disorder. When we understand the mechanisms of blushing, we can target the fear of blushing better as well.

“Aside from that, it’s interesting to know more about blushing in a general sense as well, since it happens very often and is a common part of our everyday lives.”

About this blushing and neuroimaging research news

Author: Eline Feenstra
Source: KNAW
Contact: Eline Feenstra – KNAW
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: Open access.
The Blushing Brain: Neural Substrates of Cheek Temperature Increase in Response to Self-Observation” by Christian Keysers et al. Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences


Abstract

The Blushing Brain: Neural Substrates of Cheek Temperature Increase in Response to Self-Observation

Darwin proposed that blushing—the reddening of the face owing to heightened self-awareness—is ‘the most human of all expressions’. Yet, relatively little is known about the underlying mechanisms of blushing.

Theories diverge on whether it is a rapid, spontaneous emotional response that does not involve reflection upon the self or whether it results from higher-order socio-cognitive processes. Investigating the neural substrates of blushing can shed light on the mental processes underlying blushing and the mechanisms involved in self-awareness.

To reveal neural activity associated with blushing, 16–20 year-old participants (n = 40) watched pre-recorded videos of themselves (versus other people as a control condition) singing karaoke in a magnetic resonance imaging scanner. We measured participants’ cheek temperature increase—an indicator of blushing—and their brain activity. The results showed that blushing is higher when watching oneself versus others sing.

Those who blushed more while watching themselves sing had, on average, higher activation in the cerebellum (lobule V) and the left paracentral lobe and exhibited more time-locked processing of the videos in early visual cortices.

These findings show that blushing is associated with the activation of brain areas involved in emotional arousal, suggesting that it may occur independently of higher-order socio-cognitive processes.

Our results provide new avenues for future research on self-awareness in infants and non-human animals.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.