Study Points to Possible Source of Peripheral Vision Problems

You’re hustling across Hunt­ington Avenue, eyes on the Marino Center, as the “walk” sign ticks down seconds: Seven, six, five…. Suddenly bike brakes screech to your right. Yikes! Why didn’t you see that cyclist coming with your peripheral vision?

Researchers in the lab of Northeastern psychology professor Peter J. Bex may now have the answer one that may lead to relief for those with age​​related macular degeneration, or AMD, an eye disorder that destroys central vision: the sharp, straight​​ahead vision that enables us to read, drive, and decipher faces. AMD could affect close to 3 mil­lion people by 2020, according to the Centers for Disease Control.

The problem with peripheral vision, which people with AMD particularly rely on as their central vision fails, is that it’s notoriously poor because it’s subject to “crowding,” or interference from surrounding “visual clutter,” says William J. Harrison, co​​author of the study and a former Northeastern postdoctoral fellow. “You know something’s there, but you can’t identify what it is.”

Bex and Harrison’s break­through paper, published last month in the journal Current Biology, uses computational modeling of subjects’ perceptions of images to reveal why that visual befuddlement occurs, including the brain mechanism driving it. That knowledge could pave the way for treat­ments to circumvent crowding.

An evolutionary compromise

Our vision operates on a gradient: It has high resolution in the center and progressively coarser resolution in the periphery. The diminishment is an evolutionary necessity. “If we were to have the same resolution that we have in the center of our vision across our whole visual field, we’d need a brain and an optic nerve that were at least 10 times larger than they cur­rently are,” says Bex, who specializes in basic and clinical visual science.

It’s that com­pro­mise that makes us vulnerable to crowding. “It’s not the physical properties of the eye, its shape, the number of photo receptors, that determine what we see,” says Harrison, who’s curently a post­doctoral fellow in psy­chology at the University of Cambridge. “It’s the wiring in the brain.”

Crowding, simply put, exemplifies the limited bandwidth of our visual processing system.

A new paradigm

Previous attempts to understand crowding spring from that insight. One hypothesis holds that those limited resources lead our brains to “average” the symbols in our peripheral vision, in effect adding the distractions to the target image and dividing the whole by the parts to produce an unidentifiable object. Another hypothesis suggests that each part is represented accurately in the brain’s vision center, in the occip­ital lobe. But other regions of the brain; such as the frontal lobe, which is largely responsible for visual attention; don’t have the power to select the target image from among the others.

Further blurring the picture, the prevailing hypotheses that explain crowding conflict with one another, requiring qualifiers for special cases.

Image shows a letter A and a letter A surrounded by lines.
A demonstration of visual crowding: Fixate on the green spot. Without moving your eyes, see if you can iden­tify the letter on the left and on the right side of the display. Most people will easily be able to identify the letter on the left, whereas the same letter is almost impossible to identify on the right because of crowding the brain’s propensity to combine nearby visual information into a single object. Credit: William Harrison.

But now Bex and Harrison, using an innovative experimental design, have reconciled those differences in a single computational model based on how cells in the brain’s visual center represent what we see. “Our model integrates a combination of hypotheses and enables us to find evidence for any account of crowding,” says Harrison.

Decoding the images

Most vision tests for crowding ask people to deci­pher a single letter of the alphabet, say an “A,” surrounded by lines at var­ious angles and distances, while focusing on a dot with their central vision. Hence they elicit either a right or a wrong answer (“Er, maybe it’s an “N”?).

In Bex and Harrison’s study, however, participants viewed the image of a broken ring, similar to the letter “C,” and noted where in the image the opening appeared. The mea­sure produced “an index of continuous perceptions,” says Bex, thus permitting the researchers not only to know if crowding was occur­ring but also to create a computer simulation of how the cells in the brain actually decoded the image.

“Knowing how crowding works, when it happens, and when it doesn’t, means we can start to modify the way information is presented to reduce the crowding,” says Bex. “And reducing it is what we need to do to help people with AMD.”

About this psychology research

Source: Thea Singer – Northeastern University
Image Source: The image is credited to William Harrison
Original Research: Abstract for “A Unifying Model of Orientation Crowding in Peripheral Vision” by William J. Harrison and Peter J. Bex is in Current Biology. Published online November 25 2015 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.052


Abstract

A Unifying Model of Orientation Crowding in Peripheral Vision

Highlights
•An object surrounded by distractors is unrecognizable in peripheral vision
•We provide a novel method to systematically study such “crowded” perception
•A simple population code model produces the diverse errors made by human observers
•A single mechanism may thus suffice to explain multiple classes of crowding

Summary
Peripheral vision is fundamentally limited not by the visibility of features, but by the spacing between them [ 1 ]. When too close together, visual features can become “crowded” and perceptually indistinguishable. Crowding interferes with basic tasks such as letter and face identification and thus informs our understanding of object recognition breakdown in peripheral vision [ 2 ]. Multiple proposals have attempted to explain crowding [ 3 ], and each is supported by compelling psychophysical and neuroimaging data [ 4–6 ] that are incompatible with competing proposals. In general, perceptual failures have variously been attributed to the averaging of nearby visual signals [ 7–10 ], confusion between target and distractor elements [ 11, 12 ], and a limited resolution of visual spatial attention [ 13 ]. Here we introduce a psychophysical paradigm that allows systematic study of crowded perception within the orientation domain, and we present a unifying computational model of crowding phenomena that reconciles conflicting explanations. Our results show that our single measure produces a variety of perceptual errors that are reported across the crowding literature. Critically, a simple model of the responses of populations of orientation-selective visual neurons accurately predicts all perceptual errors. We thus provide a unifying mechanistic explanation for orientation crowding in peripheral vision. Our simple model accounts for several perceptual phenomena produced by crowding of orientation and raises the possibility that multiple classes of object recognition failures in peripheral vision can be accounted for by a single mechanism.

“A Unifying Model of Orientation Crowding in Peripheral Vision” by William J. Harrison and Peter J. Bex is in Current Biology. Published online November 25 2015 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.052

Feel free to share this neuroscience article.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. This phenomenon is what I refer to as “Visual Shorthand”. Having taken numerous art and photography courses I have learned that most people are merely drawing with crayons. No one has shown them HOW to see. There is a field of study called Visual Kinematics that teaches people how to see and not just assume that they are seeing things correctly. A starting point for actually seeing what you are seeing is a beginning art class text called; “Drawing From the Right Side of the Brain”.
    The right side of the brain is responsible for creativity and processing our conceptual input. We need a certain amount of Visual Shorthand because if we were to perceive every blade of grass, every leaf on every tree our minds; every cortex in our brains would be overloaded with far too much information and short out like too many gadgets plugged into one electrical outlet.

    Observe, see and never assume.

  2. ..but that’s how Masters in Art work. They never complete lines,colour blotches, designs etc. because they want observer to fuse it together with their imagination, otherwise the artwork would be overworked, photographic dead art which is sterile of human emotions, conceptions, also to give their Art ‘freshness’. Sometimes the overdue it and not finish the work at all so it becomes semi-abstract or if extreme, pure Abstract.

    1. You may wish to pay attention to your spell check. I believe that you have misconstrued my observations. One only sees that one wishes to see.

Comments are closed.