Human Brain Recalls Visual Features in Reverse Order Than it Detects Them

Summary: A new study upends traditional models of how the brain constructs interpretations of the outside world, revealing that the brain uses a reverse order of processing to recall visual information.

Source: Zuckerman Institute.

Scientists at Columbia’s Zuckerman Institute have contributed to solving a paradox of perception, literally upending models of how the brain constructs interpretations of the outside world. When observing a scene, the brain first processes details — spots, lines and simple shapes — and uses that information to build internal representations of more complex objects, like cars and people. But when recalling that information, the brain remembers those larger concepts first to then reconstruct the details — representing a reverse order of processing. The research, which involved people and employed mathematical modeling, could shed light on phenomena ranging from eyewitness testimony to stereotyping to autism.

This study was published today in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

“The order by which the brain reacts to, or encodes, information about the outside world is very well understood,” said Ning Qian, PhD, a neuroscientist and a principal investigator at Columbia’s Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute. “Encoding always goes from simple things to the more complex. But recalling, or decoding, that information is trickier to understand, in large part because there was no method — aside from mathematical modeling — to relate the activity of brain cells to a person’s perceptual judgment.”

Without any direct evidence, researchers have long assumed that decoding follows the same hierarchy as encoding: you start from the ground up, building up from the details. The main contribution of this work with Misha Tsodyks, PhD, the paper’s co-senior author who performed this work while at Columbia and is at the Weizmann Institute of Science in Israel, “is to show that this standard notion is wrong,” Dr. Qian said. “Decoding actually goes backward, from high levels to low.”

As an analogy of this reversed decoding, Dr. Qian cites last year’s presidential election as an example.

“As you observed the things one candidate said and did over time, you may have formed a categorical negative or positive impression of that person. From that moment forward, the way in which you recalled the candidate’s words and actions are colored by that overall impression,” said Dr. Qian. “Our findings revealed that higher-level categorical decisions — ‘this candidate is trustworthy’ — tend to be stable. But lower-level memories — ‘this candidate said this or that’ — are not as reliable. Consequently, high-level decoding constrains low-level decoding.”

To explore this decoding hierarchy, Drs. Qian and Tsodyks and their team conducted an experiment that was simple in design in order to have a clear interpretation of the results. They asked 12 people to perform a series of similar tasks. In the first, they viewed a line angled at 50 degrees on a computer screen for half a second. Once it disappeared, the participants repositioned two dots on the screen to match what they remembered to be the angle of the line. They then repeated this task 50 more times. In a second task, the researchers changed the angle of the line to 53 degrees. And in a third task, the participants were shown both lines at the same time, and then had to orient pairs of dots to match each angle.

Previously held models of decoding predicted that in the two-line task, people would first decode the individual angle of each line (a lower-level feature) and the use that information to decode the two lines’ relationship (a higher-level feature).

“Memories of exact angles are usually imprecise, which we confirmed during the first set of one-line tasks. So, in the two-line task, traditional models predicted that the angle of the 50-degree line would frequently be reported as greater than the angle of the 53-degree line,” said Dr. Qian.

But that is not what happened. Traditional models also failed to explain several other aspects of the data, which revealed bi-directional interactions between the way participants recalled the angle of the two lines. The brain appeared to encode one line, then the other, and finally encode their relative orientation. But during decoding, when participants were asked to report the individual angle of each line, their brains used that the lines’ relationship — which angle is greater– to estimate the two individual angles.

“This was striking evidence of participants employing this reverse decoding method,” said Dr. Qian.

The authors argue that reverse decoding makes sense, because context is more important than details. Looking at a face, you want to assess quickly if someone is frowning, and only later, if need be, estimate the exact angles of the eyebrows. “Even your daily experience shows that perception seems to go from high to low levels,” Dr. Qian added.

Image shows dials
Visual depiction of one- and two-line tasks that participants were asked to complete and that was key to the paper’s findings. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Ning Qian/Columbia’s Zuckerman Institute.

To lend further support, the authors then constructed a mathematical model of what they think happens in the brain. They used something called Bayesian inference, a statistical method of estimating probability based on prior assumptions. Unlike typical Bayesian models, however, this new model used the higher-level features as the prior information for decoding lower-level features. Going back to the visual line task, they developed an equation to estimate individual lines’ angles based on the lines’ relationship. The model’s predictions fit the behavioral data well.

In the future, the researchers plan to extend their work beyond these simple tasks of perception and into studies of long-term memory, which could have broad implications — from how we assess a presidential candidate, to if a witness is offering reliable testimony.

“The work will help to explain the brain’s underlying cognitive processes that we employ every day,” said Dr. Qian. “It might also help to explain complex disorders of cognition, such as autism, where people tend to overly focus on details while missing important context.”

About this neuroscience research article

Funding: This research was partially supported by the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (FA9550-15-1-0439), the Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, and European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Programme under Grant Agreement No. 720270.

The authors report no financial or other conflicts of interest.

Source: Anne Holden – Zuckerman Institute
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Ning Qian/Columbia’s Zuckerman Institute.
Original Research: Abstract for “Visual perception as retrospective Bayesian decoding from high- to low-level features” by Stephanie Ding, Christopher J. Cueva, Misha Tsodyks, and Ning Qian in PNAS. Published online October 29 2017 doi:10.1073/pnas.1706906114

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]Zuckerman Institute “Human Brain Recalls Visual Features in Reverse Order Than it Detects Them.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 9 October 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/visual-recall-reversed-7696/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]Zuckerman Institute (2017, October 9). Human Brain Recalls Visual Features in Reverse Order Than it Detects Them. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved October 9, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/visual-recall-reversed-7696/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]Zuckerman Institute “Human Brain Recalls Visual Features in Reverse Order Than it Detects Them.” https://neurosciencenews.com/visual-recall-reversed-7696/ (accessed October 9, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Visual perception as retrospective Bayesian decoding from high- to low-level features

When a stimulus is presented, its encoding is known to progress from low- to high-level features. How these features are decoded to produce perception is less clear, and most models assume that decoding follows the same low- to high-level hierarchy of encoding. There are also theories arguing for global precedence, reversed hierarchy, or bidirectional processing, but they are descriptive without quantitative comparison with human perception. Moreover, observers often inspect different parts of a scene sequentially to form overall perception, suggesting that perceptual decoding requires working memory, yet few models consider how working-memory properties may affect decoding hierarchy. We probed decoding hierarchy by comparing absolute judgments of single orientations and relative/ordinal judgments between two sequentially presented orientations. We found that lower-level, absolute judgments failed to account for higher-level, relative/ordinal judgments. However, when ordinal judgment was used to retrospectively decode memory representations of absolute orientations, striking aspects of absolute judgments, including the correlation and forward/backward aftereffects between two reported orientations in a trial, were explained. We propose that the brain prioritizes decoding of higher-level features because they are more behaviorally relevant, and more invariant and categorical, and thus easier to specify and maintain in noisy working memory, and that more reliable higher-level decoding constrains less reliable lower-level decoding.

“Visual perception as retrospective Bayesian decoding from high- to low-level features” by Stephanie Ding, Christopher J. Cueva, Misha Tsodyks, and Ning Qian in PNAS. Published online October 29 2017 doi:10.1073/pnas.1706906114

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.