Predicting When a Sound Will Occur Relies on the Brain’s Motor System

Summary: Researchers from McGill University shed light on how motor signals help sharpen our ability to decipher complex sound flows.

Source: McGill University.

Whether it is dancing or just tapping one foot to the beat, we all experience how auditory signals like music can induce movement. Now new research suggests that motor signals in the brain actually sharpen sound perception, and this effect is increased when we move in rhythm with the sound.

It is already known that the motor system, the part of the brain that controls our movements, communicates with the sensory regions of the brain. The motor system controls eye and other body part movements to orient our gaze and limbs to explore our spatial environment. However, because ears are immobile it was less clear what role the motor system plays in distinguishing sounds.

Benjamin Morillon, a researcher at the Montreal Neurological Institute of McGill University, designed a study based on the hypothesis that signals coming from the sensorimotor cortex could prepare the auditory cortex to process sound, and by doing so improve its ability to decipher complex sound flows like speech and music.

Image shows the location of the auditory cortex in the brain.
The top image shows auditory melody is principally processed in the bilateral auditory cortex, with neural oscillations operating in the broad Alpha range (5-18 Hz). The bottom image shows, on the contrary, internal temporal predictions are represented in the left-lateralized sensorimotor cortex, with neural oscillations operating in the Beta (18-24 Hz) range. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Benjamin Morillon.

Working in the lab of MNI researcher Sylvain Baillet, he recruited 21 participants who listened to complex tone sequences and had to indicate whether a target melody was on average higher or lower-pitched compared to a reference. The researchers also played an intertwined distracting melody to measure the participants’ ability to focus on the target melody.

The exercise was done in two stages, one in which the participants were completely still, and another in which they tapped on a touchpad in rhythm with the target melody. The participants performed this task while their brain oscillations, a form of neural signaling brain regions use to communicate with each other, were recorded with magnetoencephalography (MEG).

MEG millisecond imaging revealed that bursts of fast neural oscillations coming from the left sensorimotor cortex were directed at the auditory regions of the brain. These oscillations occurred in anticipation of the occurrence of the next tone of interest. This finding revealed that the motor system can predict in advance when a sound will occur and send this information to auditory regions so they can prepare to interpret the sound.

One striking aspect of this discovery is that timed brain motor signaling anticipated the incoming tones of the target melody, even when participants remained completely still. Hand tapping to the beat of interest further improved performance, confirming the important role of motor activity in the accuracy of auditory perception.

“A realistic example of this is the cocktail party concept: when you try to listen to someone but many people are speaking around at the same time,” says Morillon. “In real life, you have many ways to help you focus on the individual of interest: pay attention to the timbre and pitch of the voice, focus spatially toward the person, look at the mouth, use linguistic cues, use what was the beginning of the sentence to predict the end of it, but also pay attention to the rhythm of the speech. This latter case is what we isolated in this study to highlight how it happens in the brain.”

A better understanding of the link between movement and auditory processing could one day mean better therapies for people with hearing or speech comprehension problems.

“It has implications for clinical research and rehabilitation strategies, notably on dyslexic children and hearing-impaired patients,” says Morillon. “Teaching them to better rely on their motor system by at first overtly moving in synchrony with a speaker’s pace could help them to better understand speech.”

About this neuroscience research article

Funding: The study was made possible with funding from a CIBC MNI post-doctoral fellowship for neuroimaging research to Morillon, and support to Baillet from the Killam Foundation, a Senior-Researcher Award from the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Santé (FRQS), a Discovery Grant from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the National Institutes of Health (2R01EB009048-05), and a Platform Support Grant from the Brain Canada Foundation.

Source: Shawn Hayward – McGill University
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Benjamin Morillon.
Original Research: Abstract for “Motor origin of temporal predictions in auditory attention” by Benjamin Morillon and Sylvain Baillet in PNAS. Published online October 2 2017 doi:10.1073/pnas.1705373114

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]McGill University “Predicting When a Sound Will Occur Relies on the Brain’s Motor System.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 5 October 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/motor-sound-prediction-7667/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]McGill University (2017, October 5). Predicting When a Sound Will Occur Relies on the Brain’s Motor System. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved October 5, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/motor-sound-prediction-7667/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]McGill University “Predicting When a Sound Will Occur Relies on the Brain’s Motor System.” https://neurosciencenews.com/motor-sound-prediction-7667/ (accessed October 5, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Motor origin of temporal predictions in auditory attention

In behavior, action and perception are inherently interdependent. However, the actual mechanistic contributions of the motor system to sensory processing are unknown. We present neurophysiological evidence that the motor system is involved in predictive timing, a brain function that aligns temporal fluctuations of attention with the timing of events in a task-relevant stream, thus facilitating sensory selection and optimizing behavior. In a magnetoencephalography experiment involving auditory temporal attention, participants had to disentangle two streams of sound on the unique basis of endogenous temporal cues. We show that temporal predictions are encoded by interdependent delta and beta neural oscillations originating from the left sensorimotor cortex, and directed toward auditory regions. We also found that overt rhythmic movements improved the quality of temporal predictions and sharpened the temporal selection of relevant auditory information. This latter behavioral and functional benefit was associated with increased signaling of temporal predictions in right-lateralized frontoparietal associative regions. In sum, this study points at a covert form of auditory active sensing. Our results emphasize the key role of motor brain areas in providing contextual temporal information to sensory regions, driving perceptual and behavioral selection.

“Motor origin of temporal predictions in auditory attention” by Benjamin Morillon and Sylvain Baillet in PNAS. Published online October 2 2017 doi:10.1073/pnas.1705373114

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.