Humans Use the Visual Part of Their Brain When Hearing Sounds in the Dark, Even If They Were Born Without Sight

Summary: Humans use the primary visual cortex to process sounds in the dark. This not only occurs in those with sight but also those who were born blind.

Source: Royal Holloway

Scientists have discovered that humans use the visual part of their brain when processing sounds in the dark, even if they have never had sight in their lifetime.

In a series of studies published in Current Biology, (“Decoding Natural Sounds in Early ‘Visual’ Cortex of Congenitally Blind Individuals”) an international team of researchers, led by Royal Holloway, explains how scans of brain activity from volunteers who were blindfolded and exposed to bird song, people talking and traffic noise through headphones, used their primary visual cortex to identify the sound.

The same also happened with volunteers who had been blind since birth, and thus showing that neither visual imagery nor visual experience is necessary for the primary visual cortex to decipher sounds.

Dr. Petra Vetter, from the Department of Psychology at Royal Holloway, said: “We wanted to know more of the nature of interactions between the visual and the hearing system humans have. During the study, we found that, although people with sight may use their visual imagination, those without sight, still used the same part of the brain to translate the sound, meaning that humans have a basic brain mechanism, independent from visual imagery and, more intriguingly, visual experience. The study therefore showed that even the earliest part of the visual cortex is sensitive to the information we hear, not just what we see.”

Professor Lars Muckli from University of Glasgow, who has been studying visual processing for years, added: “It is commonly assumed that visual stimuli reaching the human brain are first processed in isolation of other senses, in the ‘primary visual cortex’—a brain area that distinguishes between light and dark or between different orientations of lines. Maybe this area of the brain is processing an experience of spatial extendedness independent from whether it is communicated via the eyes or ears. Only after this first stage of processing, as the theory goes, visual information is transferred further up in the brain hierarchy, where it is linked with information from other senses. In other words, the mainstream view is that at a higher level an object can be linked to a sound, but at a lower level a line and a sound are thought to be processed separately.”

Dr. Lukasz Bola from Harvard University, who led the data analysis in this project, said: “Strikingly, we were able to identify the sounds from primary visual cortex activity in blind participants, with even greater accuracy than was achieved in sighted participants. In both blind and sighted people, sounds were better distinguished from brain activity in parts of primary visual cortex typically representing the sides of the visual field, rather than the center of the visual field. The part representing central vision—thus processing visual stimuli like letters or faces at a high resolution—seems to be less interested in sounds than the part representing the side of the visual field.”

This shows a woman standing on a dark beach
The same also happened with volunteers who had been blind since birth, and thus showing that neither visual imagery nor visual experience is necessary for the primary visual cortex to decipher sounds. Image is in the public domain.

Professor Amir Amedi from IDC Herzliya Reichman University, added: “The study also adds key evidence to an emerging theory which suggests that shaping up brain specializations are the tasks performed in each part of the brain rather than the input sense—in our case visual or auditory input to the visual system.”

The researchers emphasize the adaptive value that such early cortical interactions between vision and audition might have. The auditory signals may be very useful in preparing our visual system for what we will see in a moment or in guiding our attention to specific parts of the visual scene—especially to its sides, where vision is considerably less precise than at the center.

These mechanisms seem to be basic and evolutionarily old enough to be conserved even in people who have never seen anything. Thus, these findings show the importance of genetic blueprints in shaping the organization of the visual brain.

About this visual and auditory neuroscience research article

Source:
Royal Holloway
Contacts:
Press Office – Royal Holloway
Image Source:
The image is in the public domain.

Original Research: Closed access
“Decoding Natural Sounds in Early “Visual” Cortex of Congenitally Blind Individuals” by Petra Vetter et al. Current Biology.


Abstract

Decoding Natural Sounds in Early “Visual” Cortex of Congenitally Blind Individuals

Highlights
• Sounds can be decoded from early visual cortex activity in blind individuals
• Sound decoding accuracy increases from foveal to peripheral early visual regions
• Visual imagery is not necessary for auditory feedback to early visual cortex
• Early visual cortex organization by eccentricity develops without visual experience

Summary
Complex natural sounds, such as bird singing, people talking, or traffic noise, induce decodable fMRI activation patterns in early visual cortex of sighted blindfolded participants [1]. That is, early visual cortex receives non-visual and potentially predictive information from audition. However, it is unclear whether the transfer of auditory information to early visual areas is an epiphenomenon of visual imagery or, alternatively, whether it is driven by mechanisms independent from visual experience. Here, we show that we can decode natural sounds from activity patterns in early “visual” areas of congenitally blind individuals who lack visual imagery. Thus, visual imagery is not a prerequisite of auditory feedback to early visual cortex. Furthermore, the spatial pattern of sound decoding accuracy in early visual cortex was remarkably similar in blind and sighted individuals, with an increasing decoding accuracy gradient from foveal to peripheral regions. This suggests that the typical organization by eccentricity of early visual cortex develops for auditory feedback, even in the lifelong absence of vision. The same feedback to early visual cortex might support visual perception in the sighted and drive the recruitment of this area for non-visual functions in blind individuals.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.