Summary: A new study uncovers the different patterns and reasons behind procrastination using a mathematical framework. It shows that procrastination is influenced by factors like immediate rewards and uncertainty about future outcomes.
These insights can help develop personalized strategies to combat procrastination and improve productivity. The research also highlights the importance of understanding individual procrastination behaviors.
Key Facts:
- Procrastination Patterns: Includes behaviors like delaying or abandoning tasks.
- Brain Influences: Immediate rewards and future uncertainty drive procrastination.
- Personalized Solutions: Understanding patterns can lead to effective interventions.
Source: Max Planck Institute
Procrastination, the deliberate but detrimental deferring of tasks, has many forms. Sahiti Chebolu of the Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics uses a precise mathematical framework to understand its different patterns and their underlying reasons. Her insights could help tailor individual strategies to tackle the issue.
“Why did I not do this when I still had the time?” – Whether it is filing taxes, meeting a deadline at work, or cleaning the apartment before a family visit, most of us have already wondered why we tend to put off certain tasks, even in the face of unpleasant consequences.
Why do we make decisions that are harmful to us – against our better knowledge? This is precisely the conundrum of procrastination. Procrastination, the deliberate but ultimately detrimental delaying of tasks, is not only hampering productivity, but has also been linked to a host of mental health issues.
So it is certainly worth asking why this much talked-about phenomenon has such a grip on us – and what it actually is.
“Procrastination is an umbrella term for different behaviors,” says computational neuroscientist Sahiti Chebolu from the Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics.
“If we want to understand it, we need to differentiate between its various types.”
One common pattern is that we defect on our own decisions: we might, for example, set aside an evening for the tax return, but when the time has come we watch a movie instead. Something else is going on when we do not commit to a time in the first place: we might be waiting for the right conditions.
The possible patterns of procrastination are myriad: from starting late to abandoning a task halfway through, Chebolu classified them all and identified possible explanations for each: misjudging the time needed or protecting the ego from prospective failure are just two of them.
The short-sighted brain
Can such a classification really help you get stuff done? Chebolu is convinced that a mathematically precise understanding of the mechanism at play is the first step to tackling it. She frames procrastination as a series of temporal decisions.
What exactly happens, for example, when we schedule our tax declaration for Friday night but then succumb to the temptations of a streaming service?
One way to think of decision-making is that our brain adds up all the rewards and penalties we expect to gain from the alternative behaviors: watching a movie or doing the annoying paperwork. Quite naturally, it then picks the course of action that promises to be most pleasant overall.
But does the fun of a movie night outweigh the dismay at a hefty failure-to-file penalty? There is one important detail: consequences in the distant future are weighted less by the brain in its summation of positive and negative outcomes.
To a certain degree, this is normal and even useful; after all, the more distant future is necessarily fraught with uncertainties.
“Only when we place excessive value on experiences in the present and not enough on those lying further ahead,” Chebolu explains, “such a decision-making policy becomes quickly maladaptive.”
Just lazy?
So much for the theory. To study real-life procrastination, Chebolu delved into large datasets provided by New York University.
The data showed a log of students who were required to participate in a set number of hours of experiments over the course of a semester. Some rid themselves of the task right away; others distributed it evenly over several weeks – and, sure enough, still others shirked it until it was almost too late. Chebolu ran simulations to reproduce their behavior.
Which explanations, she asked, would best be able to account for different pattern of procrastination?
It might be tempting to lay the blame on our brain’s preference for immediately rewarding activities. But there is definitely more at play: for each pattern of how the New York students deferred their task, Chebolu found multiple possible explanations.
“Uncertainty is another major factor in procrastination,” she stresses.
This could be the failure to predict how much time we will need to unearth all receipts for deductible expenses. But uncertainty can also mean lacking confidence in our own abilities or doubting whether the task helps us achieve our goals.
Chebolu is confident that understanding procrastination as a series of temporal decisions and detecting where and why we usually take a wrong turn can inform interventions: If you discover, for instance, that your brain is a bit too biased towards instant gratification, giving yourself short-term rewards might help.
Those who tend to underestimate the time needed for their grunt work could try setting themselves time-bound goals. And if find yourself abandoning your chores quickly, you might want to avoid distracting environments.
No matter in which category of procrastination you fall (and you almost certainly fall into some of them sometimes): no, you are not just lazy. Recognizing this and forgiving yourself for procrastinating in past is a good first steps towards more productivity.
About this procrastination and behavioral neuroscience research news
Author: Daniel Fleiter
Source: Max Planck Institute
Contact: Daniel Fleiter – Max Planck Institute
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News
Original Research: Open access.
“Optimal and sub-optimal temporal decisions can explain procrastination in a real-world task” by Sahiti Chebolu et al. PsyArXiv
Abstract
Optimal and sub-optimal temporal decisions can explain procrastination in a real-world task
Procrastination is a universal phenomenon, with a significant proportion of the population reporting interference and even harm from such delays.
Why do people put off tasks despite what are apparently their best intentions, and why do they de- liberately defer in the face of prospective failure?
Past research shows that procrastination is a heterogeneous construct with possibly diverse causes.
To grapple with the complexity of the topic, we construct a taxonomy of different types of procrasti- nation and potential sources for each type.
We simulate com- pletion patterns from three broad model types: exponential or inconsistent temporal discounting, and waiting for interesting tasks; and provide some preliminary evidence, through com- parisons with real-world data, of the plausibility of multiple types of, and pathways for, procrastination.