Is Your Toddler Ready to Read?

Knowing how words differ from drawings is first step, study finds.

Even before they can read, children as young as 3 years of age are beginning to understand how a written word is different than a simple drawing — a nuance that could provide an important early indicator for children who may need extra help with reading lessons, suggests new research from Washington University in St. Louis.

“Our results show that children have some knowledge about the fundamental properties of writing from a surprisingly early age,” said study co-author Rebecca Treiman, PhD, the Burke & Elizabeth High Baker Professor of Child Developmental Psychology in Arts & Sciences.

“Based on the results, it may be possible to determine at an early age which children are progressing well in the learning of emergent literacy skills and which children may need extra attention,” Treiman said.

Published Jan. 6 in the journal Child Development, the study is based on two experiments with 114 children ages 3-to-5 years who had not yet received any formal instruction in reading or writing.

The children were tested to see how well they understood that a written word, such as dog, has one specific pronunciation (“dog”) as compared with a simple drawing of a dog, which could be correctly labeled as the image of a dog, a puppy or even a pet named Spot.

In the first test, researchers read the written word “dog” to the children.

Later, when a puppet employed in the experiment read the word “dog” as “puppy,” many children picked up on the mistake. In a similar task with drawings, children were more likely to say that the puppet was correct in using the alternative label.

The different results in the writing and drawing conditions indicate that even young pre-readers have some understanding that a written word stands for one specific linguistic unit in a way that a drawing does not. While a written word should be read the same way each time, it is sometimes appropriate to use different labels for a drawing, the researchers explain.

Most children don’t begin formal instruction in reading and writing until they turn 5 and enter kindergarten, but these findings suggest that children as young as 3 may be tested to see how well their understanding of basic language concepts is progressing.

“Our finding that preschool-age children who cannot yet read have some understanding that written words represent specific words in a way that drawings do not indicates that young children’s knowledge about the inner structure of writing — how it functions as a symbol — is more sophisticated than previously thought,” said study co-author Lori Markson, PhD, associate professor in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences in Arts & Sciences.

The results are surprising given that some literacy development theories have suggested that pre-readers treat written words as representing meanings directly, as pictures do.

More recent research, however, shows that parents often speak differently about pictures than they do about letters and words, helping even very small children begin to understand the writing something is in many ways similar to saying it.

Image shows a little girl reading a book.
Most children don’t begin formal instruction in reading and writing until they turn 5 and enter kindergarten, but these findings suggest that children as young as 3 may be tested to see how well their understanding of basic language concepts is progressing. Image adapted from the WUSTL press release.

“Such experiences may help children to learn, even before they can read, that writing conveys meaning in a different way than drawing does,” Markson said.

While dozens of research studies have shown that reading to young children helps them build a stronger cognitive foundation for later reading and writing, this study is one of the first to offer a simple method for benchmarking how well children are progressing in their understanding of basic concepts about how writing works as a symbol.

This understanding may be crucial to later success in formal reading and writing instruction.

About this neurodevelopment and learning research

Funding: This research was supported by NICHD Grant HD051610 and NSF Grant BCS-1421279. Other co-authors include Lana Hompluem, Jessica Gordon and Kristina Decker. Hompluem and Gordon worked on the research while undergraduate psychology students at Washington University.

Source: Gerry Everding – WUSTL
Image Source: The image is adapted from the WUSTL press release
Original Research: Abstract for “Young Children’s Knowledge of the Symbolic Nature of Writing” by Rebecca Treiman, Lana Hompluem, Jessica Gordon, Kristina Decker and Lori Markson in Child Development. Published online January 6 2016 doi:10.1111/cdev.12478


Abstract

Young Children’s Knowledge of the Symbolic Nature of Writing

Two experiments with one hundred and fourteen 3- to 5-year-old children examined whether children understand that a printed word represents a specific spoken word and that it differs in this way from a drawing. When an experimenter read a word to children and then a puppet used a different but related label for it, such as “dog” for the word ‹puppy›, children often stated the puppet’s label was incorrect. In an analogous task with drawings, children were more likely to state that the puppet was correct in using an alternative label. The results suggest that even young children who cannot yet read have some understanding that a written word stands for a specific linguistic unit in a way that a drawing does not.

“Young Children’s Knowledge of the Symbolic Nature of Writing” by Rebecca Treiman, Lana Hompluem, Jessica Gordon, Kristina Decker and Lori Markson in Child Development. Published online January 6 2016 doi:10.1111/cdev.12478

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. It’s an interesting study, but it’s very disappointing when “researchers” have not done their own research – have not read studies published by others in similar and relate fields, before the “researcher” makes prospective or prescriptive statements about the implications of their findings. For example, Dr. Treiman should have read about other research already conducted on the early identification of children’s development of reading skills. If she did this “research” to properly identify the contributions that her own study does or does not make to the current field of study (early literacy development in children), then she would find that other researchers have already conducted longitudinal studies to determine the best predictors of the development of reading skills in children. We already know how to screen for children who are likely to struggle to learn to read (Wagner, Torgesen, Rashotte, 1994) AND we already have multimillion dollar studies that show how to provide the most effective early intervention for children age 5 who enter Kindergarten in the bottom 12th percentile on these exact pre-literacy skills. From the work of Torgesen, et al, Conway, 1999, our team showed how to help 97.6% of these High-Risk students to develop Average Reading skills (for the bottom performing 12#% of students) – Average in Reading Accuracy, Average in Reading Fluency, etc…. We need fewer “new” studies and better cross disciplinary assimilation of existing research – otherwise research just becomes a research scientist career security product and not a mechanism for advancing the fields and services that can benefit children and adults.

Comments are closed.