Jellyfish Sleep Like the Rest of Us

Summary: Much like other animals, the upside-down jellyfish enters into sleep. However, unlike other animals, the jellyfish has no central nervous system. Researchers believe the need to sleep evolved before the emergence of a central nervous system.

Source: HHMI.

Like humans, mice, fish and flies, the upside-down jellyfish Cassiopea exhibits the telltale signs of sleep, scientists report September 21, 2017 in the journal Current Biology. But unlike other animals that slumber, jellyfish don’t have a central nervous system.

“It’s the first example of sleep in animals without a brain,” says study coauthor Paul Sternberg, a Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI) Investigator at the California Institute of Technology. The findings push the origin of sleep further down the evolutionary tree of life – to before the emergence of a centralized nervous system.

Scientists have long debated why animals sleep. Theories involve everything from memory to learning to cellular recovery. But Sternberg wanted to answer something seemingly much more basic. The big question, he says, is “Do all animals sleep?”

It’s a controversial subject. All vertebrates that scientists have studied sleep, but whether the same is true for invertebrates remains a mystery. Researchers have demonstrated that fruit flies sleep, and Sternberg and others have argued that a roundworm called Caenorhabditis elegans does too. But not everyone’s convinced. And sleep in more primitive animals — like sponges and jellyfish– is even more nebulous. “We wanted to figure this out once and for all,” Sternberg says.

a jellyfish
A Cassiopea jellyfish rests upside-down on black sand and pulses, rhythmically contracting and relaxing its bell. At night, Cassiopea jellies pulse less frequently — a clue that they’re sleeping, researchers report. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Caltech.

His graduate student, Ravi Nath, teamed up with friends at Caltech who studied Cassiopea, a mostly stationary jellyfish native to mudflats, mangrove swamps, and other warm, shallow waters. Caltech biologist Lea Goentoro kept tanks of the jellies living in artificial seawater in her lab. Cassiopea don’t look like typical jellyfish – they’re silver dollar-sized, splotched with black pigment, and rest upside-down on the sea floor. With tentacles curled above bell-shaped bodies, the jellies resemble miniature heads of cauliflower. But they pulse like other jellyfish, contracting and relaxing in a steady rhythm, and Nath and his colleagues wondered if the behavior was continuous.

“We went in at night and videotaped them with an iPhone,” he says. After recording the jellies for a minute at night and during the day and manually counting individual pulses, the team got their first clue that Cassiopea might be sleeping: They pulsed less frequently at night. The researchers then created an image processing program to count the pulses of 23 jellies over six consecutive days and nights. Pulsing activity ebbed at night, as the researchers had observed earlier, but a little food dropped in the tanks could quickly wake the jellies up again.

“It’s like the odor of coffee permeating your consciousness in the morning,” Sternberg says. Such an easy wake-up is a sign that Cassiopea‘s sluggish night-time behavior is due to sleep, not paralysis or coma.

Researchers spotted a second sign of sleep after dropping the floor out from dozing jellies. The team placed Cassiopea inside a PVC pipe with a mesh bottom and then dipped the pipe into the tank – so submerged jellies rested on the mesh instead of the tank floor. Then the researchers lowered the pipe deeper into the tank, forcing the jellies to lift off the mesh and float in open water. They don’t like that, Sternberg explains. During the day, Cassiopea will quickly pulse their bells and swim down to rest on the mesh again. But at night, it takes them about three times longer to start pulsing. It’s like the jellyfish are a little groggy, he says. This delayed response to stimulation is typical for sleeping animals.

And like other sleeping animals, if the jellies pull an all-nighter, they pay for it the next morning. Nath and colleagues pulsed tiny jets of water at Cassiopea for six or 12 hours at night, and then counted pulses the next day. They’re less active after losing sleep, Nath says, but “after they catch up on their sleep, they return to normal.”

The results suggest that Cassiopea do indeed sleep, but for Sternberg, the work is also sparking more questions. “Do you need neurons to sleep?” he asks. “Do you need more than one cell to sleep?” He’d like to study sponges, or even single cell protozoa, next and perhaps push the origin of sleep back even further.

About this neuroscience research article

Source: Bex Caygill – HHMI
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Caltech.
Video Source: Video credited to CalTech.
Original Research: Abstract for “The Jellyfish Cassiopea Exhibits a Sleep-like State” by Ravi D. Nath, Claire N. Bedbrook, Michael J. Abrams, Ty Basinger, Justin S. Bois, David A. Prober, Paul W. Sternberg, Viviana Gradinaru, Lea Goentoro in Current Biology. Published online September 21 2017 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.014

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]HHMI “Jellyfish Sleep Like the Rest of Us.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 21 September 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/sleep-jellyfish-7539/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]HHMI (2017, September 21). Jellyfish Sleep Like the Rest of Us. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved September 21, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/sleep-jellyfish-7539/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]HHMI “Jellyfish Sleep Like the Rest of Us.” https://neurosciencenews.com/sleep-jellyfish-7539/ (accessed September 21, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

The Jellyfish Cassiopea Exhibits a Sleep-like State

Highlights

•Cassiopea jellyfish exhibit reversible behavioral quiescence during the night
•Cassiopea show reduced responsiveness to stimuli during their quiescent state
•The nighttime quiescence is regulated by both homeostatic and circadian systems
•Pharmacological studies show evidence of molecular conservation of sleep regulation

Summary

Do all animals sleep? Sleep has been observed in many vertebrates, and there is a growing body of evidence for sleep-like states in arthropods and nematodes. Here we show that sleep is also present in Cnidaria, an earlier-branching metazoan lineage. Cnidaria and Ctenophora are the first metazoan phyla to evolve tissue-level organization and differentiated cell types, such as neurons and muscle. In Cnidaria, neurons are organized into a non-centralized radially symmetric nerve net that nevertheless shares fundamental properties with the vertebrate nervous system: action potentials, synaptic transmission, neuropeptides, and neurotransmitters. It was reported that cnidarian soft corals and box jellyfish exhibit periods of quiescence, a pre-requisite for sleep-like states, prompting us to ask whether sleep is present in Cnidaria. Within Cnidaria, the upside-down jellyfish Cassiopea spp. displays a quantifiable pulsing behavior, allowing us to perform long-term behavioral tracking. Monitoring of Cassiopea pulsing activity for consecutive days and nights revealed behavioral quiescence at night that is rapidly reversible, as well as a delayed response to stimulation in the quiescent state. When deprived of nighttime quiescence, Cassiopea exhibited decreased activity and reduced responsiveness to a sensory stimulus during the subsequent day, consistent with homeostatic regulation of the quiescent state. Together, these results indicate that Cassiopea has a sleep-like state, supporting the hypothesis that sleep arose early in the metazoan lineage, prior to the emergence of a centralized nervous system.

“The Jellyfish Cassiopea Exhibits a Sleep-like State” by Ravi D. Nath, Claire N. Bedbrook, Michael J. Abrams, Ty Basinger, Justin S. Bois, David A. Prober, Paul W. Sternberg, Viviana Gradinaru, Lea Goentoro in Current Biology. Published online September 21 2017 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.014

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.