Dehydration Increases Pain Perception and Reduces Blood Flow in the Brain

New research from the College of Health has shed light on the impact dehydration has on our perception of pain, and our blood pressure and brain blood flow response.

Two studies, using the same methodology but with a different focus, looked at what not drinking enough, known as hypohydration, does to our bodies.

Dr Toby Mündel, a senior lecturer in Sport and Exercise Science, says previous studies have used drugs (diuretics), exercise and heat stress to get people to lose body water in order to become dehydrated. But he says the majority of us become dehydrated simply by not drinking enough.

“Everyone has done it. You wake up late, rush to work skipping breakfast, next thing you know its lunchtime and all you have had to drink is your morning coffee. So, since dinner the night before until midday is around 17 hours and you’ve had a paltry 300 millilitres of fluid, compared to the litre-plus you are supposed to have had!”

Dr Mündel says typically studies on hydration levels also involve exercise, getting hot, restricting caffeine and altering people’s diets. “All of these things have huge effects on our mental and physical responses and so dehydration may not actually be the thing having an effect. We wanted to simplify things down to one source.”

So how were these studies carried out?

Participants visited Massey’s Manawatū campus twice, keeping their diet, activity and caffeine levels the same. The only difference was on one occasion they consumed their usual amount of fluids, while the other time they refrained from fluids for 24 hours.

On both visits, their feet were immersed in ice-cold water, known as a cold pressor test. “It’s a great way to create pain, which was the focus of one of our studies, and it’s a standard way of challenging the body and observing how it responds, like the fight or flight response, which was the focus of the second study. Plus it’s a commonly used clinical test to measure how ‘normally’ a person’s cardiovascular system is working,” says Dr Mündel.

Bachelor of Science student Tracey Bear wanted to find out whether hydration levels affect how we sense pain. Dr Mündel says this is an important area for a number of reasons. “If you have any condition where the sensation of pain is heightened, including clinical conditions like arthritis, cancer, musculoskeletal disorders, or migraines for example, or you’re preparing for or recovering from surgery where food and fluids are sometimes restricted, if dehydration were to increase your level of pain, that wouldn’t be good.

“These findings would allow medical professionals to recommend something really simple for people to manage their pain better by staying well hydrated. Also, it’s important to know common treatments like analgesics [pain-killers] or cognitive behaviour therapy, may be less effective in a dehydrated state.”

Ms Bear got participants to rate how painful it felt while their feet were immersed and also how much they worried about this pain. She found that the more dehydrated people became, the more intense the feeling of pain. However, those people already worried about pain showed less of a change.

Image shows the researchers.
Bachelor of Science student Tracey Bear and Dr Blake Perry carry out the cold pressor test on Dr Toby Mündel. Credit: Massey University.

In the second study, Massey PhD graduate Dr Blake Perry hooked participants up to a cardiovascular “rig” during the cold pressor and measured their blood pressure, heart rate and brain blood flow responses.

Dr Perry’s study found that dehydration amplified the pain of the cold pressor, which made people hyperventilate or breathe more, reducing their brain blood flow response. “While it’s difficult to fully translate the results, the take-home message is that for any clinical assessment or research setting that performs the cold pressor test, hydration level should be assessed and preferably kept the same as it may skew results,” says Dr Mündel.

About this neurodevelopment research

Student Tracey Bear received $4,000 in funding from the Neurological Foundation to complete her study. The 34-year-old now wants to undertake a PhD, and is interested in nutrition interventions for pain and mood. The next step is to look at whether this acute pain is the same as for chronic pain (like in arthritis or fibromyalgia) with a view to recommending healthy habits like good hydration. Ms Bear’s research was a collaboration with two Massey staff members from the School of Psychology, Dr Stephen Hill and Dr Michael Phillip.

Dr Blake Perry completed his Massey PhD (2014), looking at the brain blood flow response to certain types of exercise, under the supervision of Dr Toby Mündel in collaboration with Dr Sam Lucas from the University of Birmingham, UK. The 27-year-old is currently employed by Massey University, as the Human Performance Laboratory manager for the School of Sport and Exercise, based in Palmerston North.

Funding: This study was generously supported by the Massey University Research Fund and the Neurological Foundation of New Zealand. All authors contributed to the drafting of this manuscript and approved the final version.

Source: Massey University
Image Source: The image is credited to Massey University.
Original Research: Abstract for “A preliminary study on how hypohydration affects pain perception” by Tracey Bear, Michael Philipp, Stephen Hill and Toby Münde in Psychophysiology. Published online January 20 2016 doi:10.1111/psyp.12610

Full open access research for “Mild dehydration modifies the cerebrovascular response to the cold pressor test” by TBlake G. Perry, Tracey L. K. Bear, Samuel J. E. Lucas, and Toby Mündel in Experimental Physiology. Published online November 3 2015 doi:10.1113/EP085449


Abstract

A preliminary study on how hypohydration affects pain perception

Chronic pain is a prevalent health issue with one in five people suffering from some form of chronic pain, with loss of productivity and medical costs of chronic pain considerable. However, the treatment of pain can be difficult, as pain perception is complex and can be affected by factors other than tissue damage. This study investigated the effect of hypohydration (mild, voluntary dehydration from ∼24 h of limiting fluid intake, mimicking someone drinking less than usual) on a person’s pain perception. Seventeen healthy males (age 27 ± 5 years) visited the laboratory on three occasions, once as a familiarization and then twice again while either euhydrated (urine specific gravity: 1.008 ± 0.005) or hypohydrated (urine specific gravity: 1.024 ± 0.003, and −1.4 ± 0.9% body mass). Each visit, they performed a cold pressor test, where their feet were placed in cold water (0–3°C) for a maximum of 4 min. Measures of hydration status, pain sensitivity, pain threshold, and catastrophization were taken. We found that hypohydration predicted increased pain sensitivity (β = 0.43), trait pain catastrophizing, and baseline pain sensitivity (β = 0.37 and 0.47, respectively). These results are consistent with previous research, and suggest that a person’s hydration status may be an important factor in their perception of acute pain.

“A preliminary study on how hypohydration affects pain perception” by Tracey Bear, Michael Philipp, Stephen Hill and Toby Münde in Psychophysiology. Published online January 20 2016 doi:10.1111/psyp.12610


Abstract

Mild dehydration modifies the cerebrovascular response to the cold pressor test

The cold pressor test (CPT) is widely used in clinical practice and physiological research. It is characterized by a robust autonomic response, with associated increases in heart rate (HR), mean arterial pressure (MAP) and mean middle cerebral artery blood flow velocity (MCAvmean). Hydration status is not commonly reported when conducting this test, yet blood viscosity alone can modulate MCAvmean, potentially modifying the MCAvmean response to the CPT. We investigated the effect of mild dehydration on the physiological response to the CPT in 10 healthy men (mean ± SD: age 28 ± 5 years; body mass 83 ± 5 kg). All participants completed two CPTs, cold water (0°C) immersion of both feet for 90 s, with the order of the euhydration and dehydration trials counterbalanced. Beat-to-beat MCAv, MAP, HR and breath-by-breath partial pressure of end-tidal CO2 (inline image) were measured continuously. Participants’ pain perception was measured 1 min into the CPT using a visual analog scale (no pain = 0; maximal pain = 10). Dehydration significantly elevated plasma osmolality and urine specific gravity and reduced body mass (all P < 0.01). The MAP and HR responses were not different between treatments (both P > 0.05). After 90 s of immersion, the change in MCAvmean from baseline was less in the dehydration compared with the euhydration trial (change 0 ± 5 versus 7 ± 7 cm s−1, P = 0.01), as was inline image (change −3 ± 2 versus 0 ± 3 mmHg, P = 0.02). Dehydration was associated with greater relative pain sensation during the CPT (7.0 ± 1.3 vs 5.8 ± 1.8, P = 0.02). Our results demonstrate that mild dehydration can modify the cerebrovascular response to the CPT, with dehydration increasing perceived pain, lowering inline image and, ultimately, blunting the MCAvmean response.

“Mild dehydration modifies the cerebrovascular response to the cold pressor test” by TBlake G. Perry, Tracey L. K. Bear, Samuel J. E. Lucas, and Toby Mündel in Experimental Physiology. Published online November 3 2015 doi:10.1113/EP085449

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.