Segmentation and Consolidation Mechanism of Long Term Memories Identified

Summary: IDIBELL researchers have discovered a mechanism that allows us to segment experiences into memory units. Rapidly reactivating memories can lead to long term memory consolidation, they note.

Source: IDIBELL.

A study led by the Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute (IDIBELL) has identified a neural mechanism in humans that allows us to segment our experience in discrete memory units. According to the research, published in the scientific journal Current Biology, the brain identifies context changes as “frontiers” in the flow of our experience and uses them to fragment the course of events into small units of memory that can be stored long-term. The study shows that this process takes place during the identification of a boundary event thanks to the rapid reactivation of the flow of information that precedes it.

The research was carried out by Ignacio Sols, as the first author, and Lluís Fuentemilla, all of them researchers from the Cognition and Brain Plasticity Group of IDIBELL and the Neurosciences Institute of the University of Barcelona (UB). Sara DuBrow and Lila Davachi, from the department of psychology at the University of New York, in the United States, also collaborated.

Continuous experiences, discrete memories

Despite our day-to-day experiences are lived seamlessly, without any cut, scientific research has shown that changes in the context can influence the representation we make of these experiences in our memory, where they become discrete memories. “What we were interested in finding out in this case was whether this process of compartmentalization of memories begins at the same moment in which the experience is lived, and what neural mechanism could be involved. “It is known from previous studies in animals that the active neural pattern during a certain experience is reactivated once this experience ends, so the idea was to observe in a group of volunteers what happened at the brain level when a certain episode ended”, explains researcher Ignacio Soles.

The segmentation theory of events, on which the published study is based, argues that the brain acts on the grounds of constant predictions based on previous experience; When these predictions fail, for example because there is an unexpected change of context, the brain interprets this moment as a boundary event, which delimits the neural coding of the lived experiences.

“Boundary events” trigger memory encoding

To deepen this segmentation and reactivation mechanism of memories, the researchers designed an experiment in order to recreate in a simplified way these “boundary events”; the participants had to observe a sequence of images of the same category – for example, human faces – that was interrupted by an element of a different category – for example, an object.

The response of the participants was measured behaviorally using memory tests in which they were asked, given two previously visualized elements, which one they had seen first. The results of the study conclude that the elements contained in a single episode – two faces observed within a continuous sequence of faces, for example – were significantly easier to temporarily put in order than those that had been observed in different episodes – two faces shown in a sequence in which there were the images of two objects in the middle.

a brain
The segmentation theory of events, on which the published study is based, argues that the brain acts on the grounds of constant predictions based on previous experience; When these predictions fail, for example because there is an unexpected change of context, the brain interprets this moment as a boundary event, which delimits the neural coding of the lived experiences. NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.

“Episodes built from the sequential experience allow us to generate prediction models of what may happen afterwards. The context or boundary event changes would be perceived as errors in our prediction and would serve our memory system to mark the end of an episode and the possible start of a new one, and through this process, the memory system can implement, during the course of our experience, an organizational model that would impact on how the memories of our experience will be stored”, explains UB professor Lluís Fuentemilla, last author of the study.

During the experiment, the neural activity of the participants was monitored by encephalogram (EEG), a non-invasive technique that can be used to register activity in the range of milliseconds. According to Ignacio Sols, first author of the study, the analysis of the electroencephalography records confirms that “the neural patterns of the original coding of the episodic sequence are reactivated exclusively during the appearance of the corresponding boundary event, and not while the episode is unfolding. This proves that this process does not begin during an episode, but when the brain interprets that this episode has ended.”

Rapid reactivation of memories leads to long-term memory consolidation.

This research team also suggests that the segmentation of the lived episodes in packages or pieces defined by “frontier events” is a first step towards the storage of these memories in long-term memory. “The reactivation of memory is a mechanism already known in relation to the consolidation of memories, but until now it has been mainly studied as a neural mechanism that takes place during sleep, and not during the course of the experience, as we have done here”, explains Fuentemilla.

About this neuroscience research article

Source: Gemma Fornons – IDIBELL
Publisher: Organized by NeuroscienceNews.com.
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
Original Research: Abstract for “Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory” by Ignasi Sols, Sarah DuBrow, Lila Davachi, and Lluís Fuentemilla in Current Biology. Published online November 9 2017 doi:/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.057

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]IDIBELL “Segmentation and Consolidation Mechanism of Long Term Memories Identified.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 28 November 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/long-term-memory-mechanism-8046/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]IDIBELL (2017, November 28). Segmentation and Consolidation Mechanism of Long Term Memories Identified. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved November 28, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/long-term-memory-mechanism-8046/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]IDIBELL “Segmentation and Consolidation Mechanism of Long Term Memories Identified.” https://neurosciencenews.com/long-term-memory-mechanism-8046/ (accessed November 28, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory

Highlights
•We used EEG pattern similarity to study physiological mechanisms at event boundaries
•Event boundaries trigger the reinstatement of the just-encoded episode
•Pattern similarity at boundaries correlates with temporal order memory

Summary
Although everyday experiences unfold continuously over time, shifts in context, or event boundaries, can influence how those events come to be represented in memory. Specifically, mnemonic binding across sequential representations is more challenging at context shifts, such that successful temporal associations are more likely to be formed within than across contexts. However, in order to preserve a subjective sense of continuity, it is important that the memory system bridge temporally adjacent events, even if they occur in seemingly distinct contexts. Here, we used pattern similarity analysis to scalp electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings during a sequential learning task in humans and showed that the detection of event boundaries triggered a rapid memory reinstatement of the just-encoded sequence episode. Memory reactivation was detected rapidly (∼200–800 ms from the onset of the event boundary) and was specific to context shifts that were preceded by an event sequence with episodic content. Memory reinstatement was not observed during the sequential encoding of events within an episode, indicating that memory reactivation was induced specifically upon context shifts. Finally, the degree of neural similarity between neural responses elicited during sequence encoding and at event boundaries correlated positively with participants’ ability to later link across sequences of events, suggesting a critical role in binding temporally adjacent events in long-term memory. Current results shed light onto the neural mechanisms that promote episodic encoding not only for information within the event, but also, importantly, in the ability to link across events to create a memory representation of continuous experience.

“Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory” by Ignasi Sols, Sarah DuBrow, Lila Davachi, and Lluís Fuentemilla in Current Biology. Published online November 9 2017 doi:/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.057

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.