Mind Changing Can Be Risky

Summary: Leaders who use moral arguments over pragmatic ones as a basis of their position, but later change their minds, may be judged more harshly, a new study reports.

Source: American Psychological Association.

Leaders who change stance on moral position seen as hypocritical, less effective, study says.

When leaders use a moral argument rather than a pragmatic one as the basis for a position, they may be judged harshly if they change that position later. They are perceived as hypocrites, less effective and less worthy of future support, according to research published by the American Psychological Association.

“Leaders may choose to take moral stances, believing that this will improve audiences’ perceptions. And it does, initially. But all people, even leaders, have to change their minds sometimes,” said lead author Tamar Kreps, PhD, of the University of Utah. “Our research shows that leaders who change their moral minds are seen as more hypocritical, and not as courageous or flexible, compared with those whose initial view was based on a pragmatic argument. Due to this perception of hypocrisy, they are also seen as less effective and less worthy of support.”

The research was published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

In the study, Kreps and her colleagues conducted a series of 15 experiments online involving more than 5,500 participants from the United States ranging in age from 18 to 77. In each experiment, participants learned about political or business leaders who had changed their opinion on an issue. Some participants were informed that the leaders’ initial positions were based on a moral stance.

Others were told the position was based on a pragmatic argument (e.g., it was good for the economy). Across the studies, participants rated the leader who changed his or her mind on the moral stance as more hypocritical and, in most instances, less effective and worthy of their support than leaders whose initial stance was pragmatic.

What surprised the researchers the most was how difficult it was to eliminate the effect, according to Kreps. “In different studies, we tried to test various factors we thought might weaken the effect. For example, what if the leader used the same moral value in the later view as in the earlier view? What if the leader did not rely on popular support and therefore would have no reason to pander? What about participants who believed in moral relativism, the view that there is no objective reality in the first place? None of those things made a difference — initially moral mind-changers consistently seemed more hypocritical,” she said.

Image shows a man sitting at a desk.
For leaders who insist on moral arguments, there is some good news if they have to change their minds later, according to Kreps. While in all cases, leaders who changed position on a moral stance were seen as more hypocritical, if leaders framed the change as a result of a personally transformative experience or out of their control due to external forces, they were not seen as less effective or unworthy of support. NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.

Kreps believes the findings suggest that people think that breaking moral commitments is not only difficult, but also wrong. “All in all, these results paint a glum picture for initially moral leaders. When leaders take a moral position, there appears to be little they can do to avoid being perceived as hypocritical should they find they later have to change their minds,” said Kreps.

For leaders who insist on moral arguments, there is some good news if they have to change their minds later, according to Kreps. While in all cases, leaders who changed position on a moral stance were seen as more hypocritical, if leaders framed the change as a result of a personally transformative experience or out of their control due to external forces, they were not seen as less effective or unworthy of support.

“We know that moral beliefs do tend to stay more constant over time. So, leaders should take moral stances only if they have the underlying beliefs to back up those stances,” said Kreps. “Taking an inauthentic moral view to try to pander to a moralizing audience could backfire, if a leader needs to change that view later on.”

About this neuroscience research article

Source: Penny Smith – American Psychological Association
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
Original Research: Abstract for “Hypocritical Flip-Flop, or Courageous Evolution? When Leaders Change Their Moral Minds” by Kreps, Tamar A.; Laurin, Kristin; and Merritt, Anna C. in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Published online June 8 2017 doi:10.1037/pspi0000103

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]American Psychological Association “Mind Changing Can Be Risky.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 12 June 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/indecision-mind-changing-6884/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]American Psychological Association (2017, June 12). Mind Changing Can Be Risky. NeuroscienceNew. Retrieved June 12, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/indecision-mind-changing-6884/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]American Psychological Association “Mind Changing Can Be Risky.” https://neurosciencenews.com/indecision-mind-changing-6884/ (accessed June 12, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Hypocritical Flip-Flop, or Courageous Evolution? When Leaders Change Their Moral Minds

How do audiences react to leaders who change their opinion after taking moral stances? We propose that people believe moral stances are stronger commitments, compared with pragmatic stances; we therefore explore whether and when audiences believe those commitments can be broken. We find that audiences believe moral commitments should not be broken, and thus that they deride as hypocritical leaders who claim a moral commitment and later change their views. Moreover, they view them as less effective and less worthy of support. Although participants found a moral mind changer especially hypocritical when they disagreed with the new view, the effect persisted even among participants who fully endorsed the new view. We draw these conclusions from analyses and meta-analyses of 15 studies (total N = 5,552), using recent statistical advances to verify the robustness of our findings. In several of our studies, we also test for various possible moderators of these effects; overall we find only 1 promising finding: some evidence that 2 specific justifications for moral mind changes—citing a personally transformative experience, or blaming external circumstances rather than acknowledging opinion change—help moral leaders appear more courageous, but no less hypocritical. Together, our findings demonstrate a lay belief that moral views should be stable over time; they also suggest a downside for leaders in using moral framings.

“Hypocritical Flip-Flop, or Courageous Evolution? When Leaders Change Their Moral Minds” by Kreps, Tamar A.; Laurin, Kristin; and Merritt, Anna C. in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Published online June 8 2017 doi:10.1037/pspi0000103

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.