Chimps Perform Grooming Behaviors the Same Way Their Moms Did

Summary: New findings revise the current models for homeostatic control, researchers report.

Source: Harvard.

Think of all the things your mom taught you – sit up straight, close your mouth when you chew your food, remember to say please and thank you…the list goes on.

When it comes to learning how to behave, though, humans aren’t alone in looking to our Moms.

Led by Richard Wrangham, Ruth Moore Professor of Biological Anthropology, a group of researchers has shown, for the first time, that chimpanzees learn certain grooming styles from their mothers. Once learned, chimps continued to perform the behavior the same way, long after the death of their mothers. The study is described in a November 21 paper in Current Biology.

“I think what it really shows is how strong the maternal influence is,” Wrangham said. “It’s very charming, really – our oldest known son was almost 40 years old, still doing what his long-dead mother did.”

Known as “high-arm grooming,” the behavior occurs during the regular grooming sessions chimps engage in throughout the day. As two chimps groom each other, each raise one arm and either clasp hands or cross their arms as they continue to groom. Though brief – sessions average only about 45 seconds – chimps have been observed in the behavior as many as ten times a day.

But while grooming behavior is universal among chimps, high-arm grooming is not.

The behavior has been observed in eight chimp populations across Africa, Wrangham said, each of which show differing rates of clasping and non-clasping, but is notably absent in three other well-studied populations.

It’s not clear, however, whether any significance should be attached to those differences – at present, researchers have no clear answer as to why chimps engage in high-arm grooming, or what benefits they get from it.

“So what we wanted to understand was what’s responsible for the variation in palm-to-palm clasping,” Wrangham said. “When a young female joins a new group does she look at what everyone else is doing…and then do what the rest of the group does for the rest of her life?”

To find out, Wrangham and colleagues collected the most detailed data ever on how – and how often – chimps in a particular population engaged in high-arm grooming, and they quickly realized that nearly all previous theories about the behavior were wrong.

“This type of behavior, which seems so trivial in many ways – whether you clasp hands or cross arms – has been suggested as signaling membership in a group,” Wrangham said. “People often see these types of strange behaviors we see in chimps and wonder if it is some sort of group identification sign.”

“Alternatively, people have suggested that maybe it signals a special type of social relationship if two individuals do it more often with one another. But what we’re showing with this paper is that none of the obvious possibilities figure out,” Wrangham said. “The pattern varies widely within the group, it’s not closely associated with friendship, it doesn’t vary by age or sex, and it does not depend on how long an individual has been in the community.”

In fact, he said, the only connection researchers were able to identify was the maternal one. The chimpanzees are copying their mothers, not identifying with the larger community.

Like human children, young chimps learn many behaviors from their mothers, from which foods to eat to how to use tools, and – among mothers who engage in it – how to perform high-arm grooming. And since young chimps may groom almost exclusively with their mothers until about age 12, Wrangham said it’s hardly any wonder that the grooming style stays with them into adulthood.

“Even when they’re adults, even after their mothers are long dead, they still do it the same way their mother did it.” Wrangham said. “This is the first time anyone has realized this, and the pattern is delightfully clear.”

While it’s tempting to assign some larger significance to the behavior, Wrangham said studies this far haven’t been able to show whether high-arm grooming carries any social meaning.

“People may say it’s important because they stick to it,” he said. “But the alternative view is that no one in the group cares. There’s never rejection (when a clasper meets a non-clasper) – everyone has tended to think that this is something that would be seen more among individuals that have a strong alliance relationship, but it turns out to not be true.”

Image shows a chimp.
While grooming behavior is universal among chimps, high-arm grooming is not. Neurosciencenews image is for illustrative purposes only.

That finding, Wrangham said, leaves several questions still to be answered, and the hope is that further study may yet yield some insights into the behavior.

“Now that we know some families clasp hands as much as 90 percent of the time, and others do it only 10 percent, what happens when you get two individuals high-arm grooming together when one is a clasper and the other is not?” Wrangham said. “What is it that decides which pattern they will use? Is it dominance? Is it the higher ranking individual? The older one? Or maybe it is the one that initiates?

“Our current evidence suggests that neither dominance nor age carries sway. Eventually I hope we can work out the social significance of this quirky behavior. If we can find a pattern for which individual “wins” when a clasper grooms with a non-clasper, maybe we will get a clue to why individuals do high-arm grooming at all.”

About this neuroscience research article

Source: Peter Reuell – Harvard
Image Source: This NeuroscienceNews.com image is in the public domain.
Original Research: Abstract for “Distribution of a Chimpanzee Social Custom Is Explained by Matrilineal Relationship Rather Than Conformity” by Richard W. Wrangham, Kathelijne Koops, Zarin P. Machanda, Steven Worthington, Andrew B. Bernard, Nicholas F. Brazeau, Ronan Donovan, Jeremiah Rosen, Claudia Wilke, Emily Otali, and Martin N. Muller in Current Biology. Published online November 10 2016 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.005

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]Harvard. “Chimps Perform Grooming Behaviors the Same Way Their Moms Did.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 21 November 2016.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/grooming-chimp-mom-behavior-5572/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]Harvard. (2016, November 21). Chimps Perform Grooming Behaviors the Same Way Their Moms Did. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved November 21, 2016 from https://neurosciencenews.com/grooming-chimp-mom-behavior-5572/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]Harvard. “Chimps Perform Grooming Behaviors the Same Way Their Moms Did.” https://neurosciencenews.com/grooming-chimp-mom-behavior-5572/ (accessed November 21, 2016).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Distribution of a Chimpanzee Social Custom Is Explained by Matrilineal Relationship Rather Than Conformity

Highlights
•Individuals within a chimpanzee group vary widely in grooming style
•Adult chimpanzees retain their mother’s grooming styles
•Chimpanzees do not conform to the grooming style of the social community

Summary
High-arm grooming is a form of chimpanzee grooming in which two individuals mutually groom while each raising one arm. Palm-to-palm clasping (PPC) is a distinct style of high-arm grooming in which the grooming partners clasp each other’s raised palms. In wild communities, samples of at least 100 observed dyads grooming with raised hands showed PPC frequencies varying from <5% (M group, Mahale) to >30% dyads grooming (Kanyawara, Kibale), and in a large free-ranging sanctuary group, the frequency reached >80% dyads (group 1, Chimfunshi). Because between-community differences in frequency of PPC apparently result from social learning, are stable across generations, and last for at least 9 years, they are thought to be cultural, but the mechanism of transmission is unknown. Here, we examine factors responsible for individual variation in PPC frequency within a single wild community. We found that in the Kanyawara community (Kibale, Uganda), adults of both sexes varied widely in their PPC frequency (from <10% to >50%) and did not converge on a central group tendency. However, frequencies of PPC were highly consistent within matrilines, indicating that individuals maintained lifelong fidelity to the grooming style of their mothers. Matrilineal inheritance of socially learned behaviors has previously been reported for tool use in chimpanzees and in the vocal and feeding behavior of cetaceans. Our evidence indicates that matrilineal inheritance can be sufficiently strong in nonhuman primates to account for long-term differences in community traditions.

“Distribution of a Chimpanzee Social Custom Is Explained by Matrilineal Relationship Rather Than Conformity” by Richard W. Wrangham, Kathelijne Koops, Zarin P. Machanda, Steven Worthington, Andrew B. Bernard, Nicholas F. Brazeau, Ronan Donovan, Jeremiah Rosen, Claudia Wilke, Emily Otali, and Martin N. Muller in Current Biology. Published online November 10 2016 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.005

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.