Learning New Information is Easier When it is Composed of Familiar Elements

Carnegie Mellon psychologists uncover critical relationship between working memory and strength of information ‘chunks’.

People have more difficulty recalling the string of letters BIC, IAJ, FKI, RSU and SAF than FBI, CIA, JFK, IRS and USA. The well-established reason is that the amount of information we can hold in our short-term or working memory is affected by whether the information can be “chunked” into larger units.

New research by Carnegie Mellon University psychologists takes this learning principle one step further by uncovering how the strength – or familiarity – of those chunks plays a crucial role. Published in Psychonomic Bulletin Review, they show for the first time that it is easier to learn new facts that are composed of more familiar chunks.

These findings have implications for how students are taught almost any subject, including second language learning. They also indicate that the long-held belief that children have less working memory than adults may not be true because working memory resources are more rapidly consumed when the chunks are less familiar.

This novel research merges instructional innovation and brain science, two of Carnegie Mellon’s university-wide initiatives — the Simon Initiative, which aims to measurably improve student learning outcomes by harnessing decades of learning science research, and BrainHub, which focuses on how the structure and activity of the brain give rise to complex behaviors.

“We are suggesting that working memory capacity is not a fixed quantity but interacts with the familiarity of the elements that need to be processed. If everything is very familiar, it is easy to comprehend and build new knowledge. If all of the components are unfamiliar, the task becomes very difficult or impossible,” said Lynne Reder, professor of psychology in the Dietrich College of Humanities and Social Sciences and a leading expert on memory, cognition and behavior. Reder also is a member of CMU’s Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition (CNBC) and the Human-Computer Interaction Institute.

For the study, 20 CMU undergraduates with no prior experience with Chinese language learning were familiarized with 64 Chinese characters during three-hour-long sessions each week for one month. This involved searching for a particular character in visual display of similar characters. Half of the characters were randomly selected to display at a higher frequency — 20 times more often — to become more familiar to the participants, leaving the remaining “low-frequency” characters less familiar.

This shows a brain made up of cog wheels.
The findings have implications for how students are taught almost any subject, including second language learning. Credit: the researchers/Carnegie Mellon University.

Each week, participants also completed a memory test requiring them to learn an association between a pair of characters and an arbitrary English word. Each pair of characters was completely new and was composed either of the low-frequency or the high-frequency characters. The researchers found that novel combinations of characters that were made from the more familiar characters were easier to learn to associate with new information than combinations made from the less familiar characters.

Reder and her team theorized that learning would be faster and more accurate with more familiar characters because, according to them, stronger chunks consume less working memory and working memory is critical for forming new knowledge. To further support the finding that more familiar chunks consume less working memory resources, they had participants complete a popular test of working-memory (N-back) with both the high- and low-frequency characters and found that the N-back task was much easier with the high-frequency characters.

This image shows cards with different symbols on them.
The researchers found that novel combinations of characters that were made from the more familiar characters were easier to learn to associate with new information than combinations made from the less familiar characters. Credit: the researchers/Carnegie Mellon University.

“This work has implications for how to optimize instruction, specifically that concepts should be introduced to students in a way that they have a good grasp and familiarity with those concepts before trying to combine them into more complex informational structures. These findings may also help to explain certain paradoxes such as why children tend to learn computer applications more easily than adults and may help to explain why they learn second languages better than adults,” Reder said.

“Little kids may actually have more working memory than adults. They often appear to have less only because they have fewer knowledge chunks and those chunks are weaker than adults. Adults have wisdom ‘knowledge and skills — and scientists have been confusing that with greater working memory,” she said.

About this learning and memory research

In addition to Reder, the research team included CMU’s Xiaonan Liu, Alexander Keinath and Vencislav Popov.

Funding: This study was partially supported by the National Institute of Mental Health.

Source: Shilo Rea – Carnegie Mellon University
Image Source: The images are credited to the researchers/Carnegie Mellon University
Original Research: Abstract for “Building knowledge requires bricks, not sand: The critical role of familiar constituents in learning” by Lynne M. Reder, Xiaonan L. Liu, Alexander Keinath, and Vencislav Popov in Psychonomic Bulletin Review. Published online July 3 2015 doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0889-1


Abstract

Building knowledge requires bricks, not sand: The critical role of familiar constituents in learning

Despite vast efforts to better understand human learning, some principles have been overlooked; specifically, that less familiar stimuli are more difficult to combine to create new knowledge and that this is because less familiar stimuli consume more working memory resources. Participants previously unfamiliar with Chinese characters were trained to discriminate visually similar characters during a visual search task over the course of a month, during which half of the characters appeared much more frequently. Ability to form associations involving these characters was tested via cued recall for novel associations consisting of two Chinese characters and an English word. Each week performance improved on the cued-recall task. Crucially, however, even though all Chinese character pairs were novel each week, those pairs consisting of more familiar characters were more easily learned. Performance on a working-memory task was better for more familiar stimuli, consistent with the claim that familiar stimuli consume fewer working memory resources. These findings have implications for optimal instruction, including second language learning.

“Building knowledge requires bricks, not sand: The critical role of familiar constituents in learning” by Lynne M. Reder, Xiaonan L. Liu, Alexander Keinath, and Vencislav Popov in Psychonomic Bulletin Review. Published online July 3 2015 doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0889-1

Feel free to share this neuroscience article.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.