College Students Study Better Later in the Day

Summary: A new study reports the best times for college students to learn in between 11 am and 9:30 pm.

Source: University of Nevada, Reno.

Students learn more effectively between 11 a.m. and 9:30 p.m. than at other times of the day.

A new cognitive research study used two new approaches to determine ranges of start times that optimize functioning for undergraduate students. Based on a sample of first and second year university students, the University of Nevada, Reno and The Open University in the United Kingdom used a survey-based, empirical model and a neuroscience-based, theoretical model to analyse the learning patterns of each student to determine optimum times when cognitive performance can be expected to be at its peak.

“The basic thrust is that the best times of day for learning for college-age students are later than standard class hours begin,” Mariah Evans, associate professor of sociology at the University of Nevada, Reno and co-author of the study, said. “Especially for freshmen and sophomores, we should be running more afternoon and evening classes as part of the standard curriculum.”

Prior research has demonstrated that late starts are optimal for most high school students, and this study extends that analysis to freshmen and sophomores in college. The analysis by Evans, Jonathan Kelley, fellow University sociology professor, and Paul Kelley, honorary associate of sleep, circadian and memory neuroscience at The Open University, assessed the preferred sleeping times of the participants and asked them to rate their fitness for cognitive activities in each hour of the 24-hour day.

“Neuroscientists have documented the time shift using biological data – on average, teens’ biologically ‘natural’ day begins about two hours later than is optimal for prime age adults,” Evans said. “The survey we present here support that for college students, but they also show that when it comes to optimal performance, no one time fits all.”

The study showed that much later starting times of after 11 a.m. or noon, result in the best learning. It also revealed that those who saw themselves as “evening” people outnumbered the “morning” people by 2:1, and it concluded that every start time disadvantages one or more of the chronotypes (propensity for the individual to be alert and cognitively active at a particular time during a 24-hour period).

Image shows a college campus at night.
New University of Nevada, Reno and The Open University research study used two new approaches to determine ranges of the best times of day for learning for college-age students. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to University of Nevada, Reno.

“Thus, the science supports recent moves by the University to encourage evening classes as part of the standard undergraduate curriculum,” Evans said. “It also supports increasing the availability of asynchronous online classes to enable students to align their academic work times with their optimal learning times.”

The results, Identifying the best times for cognitive functioning using new methods: Matching university times to undergraduate chronotypes, were published in Frontiers in Human Neuroscience March 31, 2017.

“This raises the question as to why conventional universities start their lectures at 9 a.m. or even earlier when our research reveals that this limits the performance of their students,” Kelley said. “This work is very helpful for asynchronous online learning as it allows for the student to target their study time to align with their personal rhythm and at the time of day when they know they are most effective.”

About this neuroscience research article

Source: Natalie Savidge – University of Nevada, Reno
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to University of Nevada, Reno.
Original Research: Abstract for “Identifying the best times for cognitive functioning using new methods: Matching university times to undergraduate chronotypes” by M.D.R. Evans, Paul Kelley and Johnathan Kelley in Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. Published online April 2017 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00188

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]University of Nevada, Reno “College Students Study Better Later in the Day.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 12 April 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/college-study-time-6394/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]University of Nevada, Reno (2017, April 12). College Students Study Better Later in the Day. NeuroscienceNew. Retrieved April 12, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/college-study-time-6394/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]University of Nevada, Reno “College Students Study Better Later in the Day.” https://neurosciencenews.com/college-study-time-6394/ (accessed April 12, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Identifying the best times for cognitive functioning using new methods: Matching university times to undergraduate chronotypes

University days generally start at fixed times in the morning, often early morning, without regard to optimal functioning times for students with different chronotypes. Research has shown that later starting times are crucial to high school students’ sleep, health, and performance. Shifting the focus to university, this study used two new approaches to determine ranges of start times that optimize for undergraduates. The first is a survey-based, empirical model (SM), and the second a neuroscience-based, theoretical model (NM). The SM focused on students’ self-reported chronotype and times they feel at their best. Using this approach, data from 190 mostly first and second year university students were collected and analyzed to determine optimal times when cognitive performance can be expected to be at its peak. The NM synthesized research in sleep, circadian neuroscience, sleep deprivation’s impact on cognition and practical considerations to create a generalized solution to determine the best learning hours. Strikingly the SM and NM results align with each other and confirm other recent research in indicating later start times. They add several important points: 1) They extend our understanding by showing that much later starting times (after 11am or 12 noon) are optimal; 2) Every single start time disadvantages one or more chronotypes; and 3) The best practical model may involve three alternative starting times with one afternoon shared session. The implications are briefly considered.

“Identifying the best times for cognitive functioning using new methods: Matching university times to undergraduate chronotypes” by M.D.R. Evans, Paul Kelley and Johnathan Kelley in Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. Published online April 2017 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00188

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.