Deprivation in Childhood Linked to Impulsive Behavior in Adulthood

Summary: Findings reveal a link between childhood deprivation and impulsivity and addiction later in life.

Source: Aston University

Children who have experienced deprivation are more likely to make more impulsive choices than those who don’t and can lead to addictions in later life – research has shown.

‘Trait impulsivity’, the preference for immediate gratification, has been linked to spending more on food, especially unhealthy, highly calorific food. Studies have shown that children who experience poverty and food insecurity tend to have a higher body-mass index as adults than those who do not.

Researchers from the School of Psychology at Aston University found a link between deprivation in childhood and impulsive behavior – leading to addictions later in life. The findings, which are a culmination of six years of research, also found a further link between impulsivity, obesity and the cost of living crisis.

Professor Richard Tunney, head of the School of Psychology at Aston University, published a study in Scientific Reports earlier this year where he showed that children who experience deprivation make more impulsive choices than children who don’t.

The research team studied 146 children, with an average age of eight, living in some of the most deprived areas of England and compared them with children living in some of the most affluent neighbourhoods.

Children were given a choice between taking home a small amount of money (for example, £1) or getting £10 a week, or even more a year later. How long a person is willing to wait for the larger amount of money can be used to calculate a ‘discount rate’ that shows how much the waiting time reduces the value of the money.

An impulsive person might prefer £1 now because the value of £10 in six months is ‘discounted’ to less than £1 right now. This means that, for them, the £10, is discounted by £9 over the six-month wait.

A less impulsive person might be willing to wait six months for £10, but not wait for a whole year for £15. This means that, for them, the value of the £15 is discounted by £5 over the additional six-month wait. This discount rate is a measure of how impulsive someone is.

Commenting on the findings, Professor Tunney said: “The results showed that children living in the most deprived areas had significantly higher discount rates than children living in the least deprived areas, regardless of age or intelligence, indicating that deprivation was the causal factor in the children’s choice.

“This preference for immediate outcomes is a stable personality trait that remains constant throughout a person’s life.”

However, in the research team’s most recent study published by the Royal Society, they investigated impulsivity in over 1,000 older adults aged between 50 and 90.

The study found that older adults living in the most deprived areas showed the same preference for smaller-sooner financial outcomes as the children in the first study.

It also found that a person’s job predicted the choices they made. Adults working in technical or routine occupations, such as mechanics or cleaners, chose to receive smaller amounts of money than wait for larger amounts compared with people in professional occupations, such as engineers or scientists.

This shows a sad little boy
The study found that older adults living in the most deprived areas showed the same preference for smaller-sooner financial outcomes as the children in the first study. Image is in the public domain

Professor Tunney added: “These findings are concerning because impulsivity doesn’t just predict obesity. These findings tell us a lot about why people living in poorer areas tend to be unhealthier than people living in wealthy areas.

“People who experience deprivation as children are more likely to choose to do things that, although they might be pleasurable in the short term, are unhealthy in the long run. This includes overeating, taking drugs, smoking cigarettes and gambling.

“We know too, that impulsivity can help to explain why some people go on to become addicts, while other people can avoid some of the more harmful effects of drugs and alcohol.

“Deprivation is one of many factors that can lead to impulsive behaviour throughout a person’s lifetime. Genetics also plays a role in impulsivity. Policymakers can’t do anything about a person’s genes but they can influence the nation’s long-term mental and physical health by minimising child poverty. Failing to do so will have long-term implications for the children living through today’s cost of living crisis.”

About this psychology research news

Author: Rebecca Hume
Source: Aston University
Contact: Rebecca Hume – Aston University
Image: The image is in the public domain

Original Research: Open access.
Individual differences in decision-making: evidence for the scarcity hypothesis from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing” by Richard Tunney et al. Royal Society Open Science


Abstract

Individual differences in decision-making: evidence for the scarcity hypothesis from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing

We report the results of a pre-registered analysis of data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing that was designed to test the hypothesis that economic scarcity is associated with individual differences in decision-making.

We tested this hypothesis by comparing time preferences for different socio-economic groups and in geographical areas ranging from the most deprived to the least deprived in England using the English indices of multiple deprivation.

The data supported this hypothesis: people in the most deprived areas were more likely to prefer smaller-sooner rewards than people from the least deprived areas.

Similarly, people in technical or routine occupations tended to prefer smaller-sooner rewards than people in professional or intermediate occupations. In addition, we found that gender, cognitive function and subjective social status also predicted time preferences.

We discuss these results in the context of theoretical models of scarcity-based models of choice behaviour and decision-making.

Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.
  1. I have been having mood swing with just stoped seriqwil not shur if I should have i took my new girlfriend s advice.is there any other options for me?

  2. This is ridiculous. Obviously the subjects needed the money right away to survive and did not have the luxury of waiting for more. That’s life. What idiot actually got this published as “news” lol

  3. This study is interesting, buy seems, at least from the synopsis in this article, to have some gaping holes.
    Couldn’t it be simple mathematics, that if you have money in the bank, you can afford to wait to let that money grow, but if you don’t, it could make a difference now? Were people who lived in poverty as children, but in wealth as adults used as a control? Or, viceversa? That would go a long way to telling you if the conclusions you came to were true.

  4. This is one link I had not considered, that wealthier people are healthier because of less impulsive behavior. I assumed it was because cheap food being cheap food caused poor health.

  5. This is fantastically shit, you have no remotely isolated deprivation as the causal factor nor have you remotely scaled the experiment to make the long term choices relevant. 10x increase over six months and only 50% increase to double the time? You will not be able to replicate this study, to someone living in poverty, the promise of a pittance in a year’s time is insulting, not evidence of delayed gratification. Absolutely dim witted this whole thing.

Comments are closed.