Brain Imaging Results Skewed by Biased Study Samples

Summary: Researchers say failure to follow principles of population science can skew results of brain imaging studies.

Source: UCSF.

Neuroimaging researchers must consider who their samples represent, UCSF study says.

Any scientist who studies groups of people knows that the characteristics of the “sample” — the group of people selected for the study — can profoundly impact the study’s findings. To produce the most accurate findings, a study group ought to be as similar as possible to the people in the larger population you want to say something about.

A new UC San Francisco-led study shows that failure to follow this basic principle of population science — a common complaint about research in the cognitive sciences — can profoundly skew the results of brain imaging studies, leading to errors that may be throwing off neuroscientists’ understanding of normal brain development.

“Much of what we know about how the brain develops comes from samples that don’t look like the broader U.S. at all,” said Kaja LeWinn, ScD, an epidemiologist and assistant professor of psychiatry at UCSF, member of the UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences, and lead author of the new study. “We would never try to understand the burden of other health conditions, like cardiovascular disease, in a sample with much higher socioeconomic status than the U.S. population as a whole, for instance.”

In recent years, social scientists have drawn attention to the over-representation of so-called W.E.I.R.D (White, Educated, Industrial, Rich, Developed) individuals in cognitive science experiments, but LeWinn says her team’s new study — published online October 12, 2017 in Nature Communications — is the first to look directly at exactly how these sampling practices impact the research results of neuroimaging studies.

The research team — which included Margaret A. Sheridan, PhD, of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Katherine M. Keyes, PhD, and Ava Hamilton of Columbia University; and Katie A. McLaughlin, PhD, of the University of Washington — found that applying the sampling approaches frequently used in brain imaging studies to a large dataset of pediatric MRI images significantly distorted findings about how different regions of the brain develop with age.

“These findings give us pause because they raise questions about existing knowledge of brain development in children, which is based almost entirely on non-representative samples,” LeWinn said. “These foundational studies inform our understanding not only of brain development, but also of mental health disorders like depression and autism. It will be critical for the field to evaluate whether these findings replicate in larger samples that better resemble the US population in all its diversity.”

The new findings are also likely to have broad implications for understanding why it is often difficult to reproduce research results in human neuroscience, LeWinn added. “The field has had a hard time replicating findings in neuroimaging, but so far, explanations for that have been focused on the way that brain imaging data are processed and analyzed, not on sample composition. But if we conducted the exact same study on two groups of people with completely different underlying characteristics, there is no reason to expect that we would find the same answer.”

The Developing Brain Changes With Age — and With Study Samples

The authors made use of a large, publicly available dataset of brain scans from 1162 children aged three to 18 years from the Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition and Genetics Study (PING). As is typical of developmental neuroimaging studies, the demographics of the PING sample were not very similar to the wider U.S. population. For example, household education and income levels were considerably higher in the children in this dataset than in the broader U.S. Typically, investigators would use PING data without considering these demographic differences, LeWinn said.

In the new study LeWinn and her colleagues created a “weighted” version of this dataset that better reflected the distribution of demographic characteristics such as sex, race/ethnicity, parental education and income in the US population, and then asked whether correctly weighting the sample changed what the brain images revealed about how the brain develops during childhood.

The researchers found that weighting the data in this fashion significantly altered what the brain images showed about the timeline of brain development during childhood and adolescence. In most cases, the weighted data showed brain regions reaching their peak size much earlier than the unweighted data would have suggested, suggesting a faster pace of development in the weighted sample. For example, the unweighted data showed overall brain surface area peaking at 12.1 years , while the correctly weighted data revealed that this peak more likely occurs at 9.7 years of age — a 2.4 year difference.

The authors also found that weighting the PING dataset impacted the apparent order in which different brain regions develop. In the unweighted sample, the frontal, occipital, and temporal lobes of the brain all seem to develop at about the same time, which is contrary to a broader understanding in neuroscience that the brain develops from back to front, starting with sensory and motor regions and only in late adolescence finishing the development of regions in the front of the brain involved in more abstract forms of cognition. The weighted imaging data matched this back-to-front model much better: it showed the parietal and occipital lobes in the back of the head developing earliest, followed by the temporal lobe beneath the ears, and the frontal lobe last.

Image shows a single image of a human brain using a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) machine. A bright blue color where brain cancer metastasizes in the occipital lobe.
The new research adds to a growing debate in neuroscience about the use of small “samples of convenience” rather than investing in larger and more sweeping samples that carefully represent well-defined target populations of interest within the U.S. NeuroscienceNews.com image is credited to Dr. Leon Kaufman. University Of California, San Francisco/National Cancer Institute. For illustrative purposes only.

“The differences we found are pretty dramatic,” said LeWinn. “The weighted data showed maturation peaking in some brain regions as much as four years earlier than the unweighted data would have suggested. We’ve shown that simply by weighting a dataset to better approximate a representative U.S. sample, we can end up with very different ideas about how the brain develops. Not only do we see big differences in age at peak surface area and volume, but we also found that the weighted data gives us a pattern of brain development that is more consistent with what we’d expect, with sensorimotor areas developing first, followed by association cortices.”

Enter Population Neuroscience

The new research adds to a growing debate in neuroscience about the use of small “samples of convenience” rather than investing in larger and more sweeping samples that carefully represent well-defined target populations of interest within the U.S.

“Most cognitive neuroscience studies are so small that we can’t apply the weighting methods used in our study to approximate a U.S. sample, which we were able to do because of the large size of the PING dataset,” LeWinn said. “But at a minimum, researchers doing these studies need to be very transparent about the demographic characteristics of their sample, and consider to whom it’s reasonable to generalize their findings. That way when the community sees inconsistent findings across studies of the same research question, we can consider differences in sample composition as one potential explanation.”

LeWinn added that it’s not reasonable to insist that all neuroimaging studies of healthy populations be perfectly representative of the U.S: “That would be impractical and financially prohibitive, and we certainly don’t want to suggest that. But certainly for large, multisite studies, like the new NIH-funded ABCD [Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development] study, which represents a major investment of taxpayer dollars, representative sampling should be a primary goal.”

About this neuroscience research article

Funding: The research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, the Jacobs Foundation, the AIM for Mental Health program of One Mind Institute, and the Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition and Genetics Study, which is funded by the National Institutes of Health, the National Institute on Drug Abuse and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development.

Source: Nicholas Weiler – UCSF
Image Source: NeuroscienceNews.com images is credited to Dr. Leon Kaufman. University Of California, San Francisco/National Cancer Institute and is in the public domain.
Original Research: Full open access research for “Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure” by Kaja Z. LeWinn, Margaret A. Sheridan, Katherine M. Keyes, Ava Hamilton & Katie A. McLaughlin in Nature Communications. Published online October 12 2017 doi:10.1038/s41467-017-00908-7

Cite This NeuroscienceNews.com Article

[cbtabs][cbtab title=”MLA”]UCSF “Brain Imaging Results Skewed by Biased Study Samples.” NeuroscienceNews. NeuroscienceNews, 12 October 2017.
<https://neurosciencenews.com/brain-imaging-biased-7731/>.[/cbtab][cbtab title=”APA”]UCSF (2017, October 12). Brain Imaging Results Skewed by Biased Study Samples. NeuroscienceNews. Retrieved October 12, 2017 from https://neurosciencenews.com/brain-imaging-biased-7731/[/cbtab][cbtab title=”Chicago”]UCSF “Brain Imaging Results Skewed by Biased Study Samples.” https://neurosciencenews.com/brain-imaging-biased-7731/ (accessed October 12, 2017).[/cbtab][/cbtabs]


Abstract

Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure

Despite calls to incorporate population science into neuroimaging research, most studies recruit small, non-representative samples. Here, we examine whether sample composition influences age-related variation in global measurements of gray matter volume, thickness, and surface area. We apply sample weights to structural brain imaging data from a community-based sample of children aged 3–18 (N = 1162) to create a “weighted sample” that approximates the distribution of socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, and sex in the U.S. Census. We compare associations between age and brain structure in this weighted sample to estimates from the original sample with no sample weights applied (i.e., unweighted). Compared to the unweighted sample, we observe earlier maturation of cortical and sub-cortical structures, and patterns of brain maturation that better reflect known developmental trajectories in the weighted sample. Our empirical demonstration of bias introduced by non-representative sampling in this neuroimaging cohort suggests that sample composition may influence understanding of fundamental neural processes.

“Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure” by Kaja Z. LeWinn, Margaret A. Sheridan, Katherine M. Keyes, Ava Hamilton & Katie A. McLaughlin in Nature Communications. Published online October 12 2017 doi:10.1038/s41467-017-00908-7

Feel free to share this Neuroscience News.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.