Independent Working Memory Systems in Animals

Data could inform new pre-clinical research models for Alzheimer’s disease treatment that goes beyond spatial memory.

A new study from Indiana University could help ensure the hundreds of millions of dollars spent each year to develop potential treatments for Alzheimer’s disease aren’t wasted on targeting the wrong types of memory.

The paper, published today in Current Biology, is the first to confirm that a key aspect of human memory impaired in memory disorders exists in the type of pre-clinical animal models that influence major decisions about drug development.

The study’s results, which required over a year’s worth of intensive data collection and analysis, could hold important insights for drug companies. That’s because selecting less relevant data early in the research process can create costly errors later in the “translational pipeline” that connects basic science to new treatments and therapies.

“There is a huge history of translation failure in memory disorders caused by companies trying to develop compounds based on therapies that produce relief in pre-clinical animal models but later fail during early clinical trials,” said Jonathon Crystal, professor in the IU Bloomington College of Arts and Sciences’ Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences and director of the Program in Neuroscience, who led the study. “We’re working to create stronger pre-clinical models of the types of memory systems that are impaired in human diseases.”

The conclusions are notable in light of the federal government’s recent 60 percent increase in Alzheimer’s disease research funding, or $350 million in new spending. No treatment currently exists to halt or reverse the long-term effects of Alzheimer’s disease, estimated to affect 5.3 million people in the United States alone.

The IU study, conducted in rats, shows for the first time that the animals possess two independent “working memory” resources, or the ability to remember more information across two categories versus a single category. In humans, working memory consists of two memory resources: visual and auditory information. The average person, for example, cannot recall a phone number longer than seven characters despite easily remembering both the audio and video on a television show.

To test these forms of memory in animals, Crystal’s team challenged rats to memorize odors and spatial information. To test rats’ ability to remember spatial data, IU scientists had them find food pellets inside an eight-arm maze. To test their ability to remember new odors, they used pellets inside containers scented by up to 100 common household spices, with only new odors yielding food.

Across numerous trials, IU scientists consistently showed that the rats could recall significantly more details in combination — scents and spaces — compared to trying to remember a single type of information.

“We saw high-level performance because the animals were encoding information in two dedicated memory resources,” Crystal said. “This is the defining quality of working memory in people; and for the first time, we’ve shown animals have this property of independent memory systems as well.”

The results also suggest that this form of memory arose evolutionarily much earlier than previously thought.

Historically, Crystal said almost all investigations on the genetics of Alzheimer’s disease depend upon spatial memory research because these studies are easier to carry out. Yet treatments based solely upon spatial memory data aren’t likely to strike at the heart of what’s so cruel about memory disorders. It’s critical to also investigate more complex forms of memory, including working memory.

Image shows a mouse sitting in spices.
This image shows a rat with several of the 100 common household spices used to test odor memory in the study. Credit: Indiana University.

“What researchers are doing now is akin to coming up with a plan for developing a drug which, if successful after spending billions of dollars, helps your grandmother find her reading glasses or car keys,” Crystal said. “Those symptoms aren’t the most debilitating aspect of Alzheimer’s disease. We need solutions that address the inability to remember significant things, like memories of the past or personal exchanges with friends and family, whose loss is so distressing to sufferers of the disease and their loved ones.”

The IU study was made possible in part by student researchers from the College of Arts and Sciences whose work helped overcome the significant time investment required to perform complex memory trials in animals.

IU students who contributed to the project were first author Alexander Bratch, now a graduate student at the University of Minnesota, who wrote his undergraduate thesis on the work; Diana Arman, Austin Dunn, Shiloh Cooper, Hannah Corbin, Stefan Dalecki and Spencer Kann, all undergraduates, and Alexandra Smith, a graduate student.

About this memory research

Also authors on the paper were Joshua Cain of Drake University; Nilda Rivera-Reyes of the University of Puerto Rico, who contributed to the research through the IU Center for Integrative Study of Animal Behavior’s Summer Research Experience for Undergraduates program; and Jie-En Wu, a student at Bloomington North High School who contributed to the project over the summer through Project STEM. Dalecki’s participation was supported by the IU Science, Technology and Research Scholars Program.

Additional IU authors were Amanda R. Doyle and Matthew J. Pizzo, both research associates in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences.

Funding: This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health, National Institute on Aging and National Science Foundation.

Source: Kevin Fryling – Indiana University
Image Credit: The image is credited to Indiana University
Original Research: Abstract for “Working Memory Systems in the Rat” by Alexander Bratch, Spencer Kann, Joshua A. Cain, Jie-En Wu, Nilda Rivera-Reyes, Stefan Dalecki, Diana Arman, Austin Dunn, Shiloh Cooper, Hannah E. Corbin, Amanda R. Doyle, Matthew J. Pizzo, Alexandra E. Smith, and Jonathon D. Crystal in Current Biology. Published online January 14 2016 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.068


Abstract

Working Memory Systems in the Rat

Highlights
•Olfactory memory is resistant to interference from adding a spatial memory load
•Spatial memory is resistant to interference from adding an olfactory memory load
•Olfactory and spatial memory draw on independent working memory systems in the rat
•Independence of working memory systems is evolutionarily quite old

Summary
A fundamental feature of memory in humans is the ability to simultaneously work with multiple types of information using independent memory systems. Working memory is conceptualized as two independent memory systems under executive control [ 1, 2 ]. Although there is a long history of using the term “working memory” to describe short-term memory in animals, it is not known whether multiple, independent memory systems exist in nonhumans. Here, we used two established short-term memory approaches to test the hypothesis that spatial and olfactory memory operate as independent working memory resources in the rat. In the olfactory memory task, rats chose a novel odor from a gradually incrementing set of old odors [ 3 ]. In the spatial memory task, rats searched for a depleting food source at multiple locations [ 4 ]. We presented rats with information to hold in memory in one domain (e.g., olfactory) while adding a memory load in the other domain (e.g., spatial). Control conditions equated the retention interval delay without adding a second memory load. In a further experiment, we used proactive interference [ 5–7 ] in the spatial domain to compromise spatial memory and evaluated the impact of adding an olfactory memory load. Olfactory and spatial memory are resistant to interference from the addition of a memory load in the other domain. Our data suggest that olfactory and spatial memory draw on independent working memory systems in the rat.

“Working Memory Systems in the Rat” by Alexander Bratch, Spencer Kann, Joshua A. Cain, Jie-En Wu, Nilda Rivera-Reyes, Stefan Dalecki, Diana Arman, Austin Dunn, Shiloh Cooper, Hannah E. Corbin, Amanda R. Doyle, Matthew J. Pizzo, Alexandra E. Smith, and Jonathon D. Crystal in Current Biology. Published online January 14 2016 doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.068

Feel free to share this neuroscience news.
Join our Newsletter
I agree to have my personal information transferred to AWeber for Neuroscience Newsletter ( more information )
Sign up to receive our recent neuroscience headlines and summaries sent to your email once a day, totally free.
We hate spam and only use your email to contact you about newsletters. You can cancel your subscription any time.